Născut sau făcut / Innato o adquirido

Page started at 29.01.2020_02.55 (UTC+1 / España)

Fuente / Sursa: Innato o adquirido (Naturaleza vs crianza);

Opinión admin / Opinie admin: "Înnăscut vs dobândit" = "Născut vs făcut"

Traducción, adaptación, interpretación... por admin.

Traducere, adaptare, interpretare... admin.

The nature versus nurture debate involves whether human behavior is determined by the environment, either prenatal or during a person's life, or by a person's genes. The alliterative expression "nature and nurture" in English has been in use since at least the Elizabethan period and goes back to medieval French.

The combination of the two concepts as complementary is ancient (Greek: ἁπό φύσεως καὶ εὐτροφίας). Nature is what we think of as pre-wiring and is influenced by genetic inheritance and other biological factors. Nurture is generally taken as the influence of external factors after conception e.g. the product of exposure, experience and learning on an individual.

The phrase in its modern sense was popularized by the English Victorian polymath Francis Galton, the modern founder of eugenics and behavioral genetics, discussing the influence of heredity and environment on social advancement. Galton was influenced by the book On the Origin of Species written by his half-cousin, Charles Darwin.

The view that humans acquire all or almost all their behavioral traits from "nurture" was termed tabula rasa ("blank slate") by John Locke in 1690. A "blank slate view" in human developmental psychology, one that assumes that human behavioral traits develop almost exclusively from environmental influences (sometimes termed "blank-slatism"), was widely held during much of the 20th century. The debate between "blank-slate" denial of the influence of heritability, and the view admitting both environmental and heritable traits, has often been cast in terms of nature versus nurture. These two conflicting approaches to human development were at the core of an ideological dispute over research agendas throughout the second half of the 20th century. As both "nature" and "nurture" factors were found to contribute substantially, often in an inextricable manner, such views were seen as naive or outdated by most scholars of human development by the 2000s.[8][9][10][11][12][13]

The strong dichotomy of nature versus nurture has thus been claimed to have limited relevance in some fields of research. Close feedback loops have been found in which "nature" and "nurture" influence one another constantly, as seen in self-domestication. In ecology and behavioral genetics, researchers think nurture has an essential influence on nature.[14][15] Similarly in other fields, the dividing line between an inherited and an acquired trait becomes unclear, as in epigenetics[16] or fetal development.[17][18]

Contents

1 History of the debate

2 Heritability estimates

3 Interaction of genes and environment

3.1 Social pre-wiring

3.2 Obligate vs. facultative adaptations

3.3 Advanced techniques

3.4 Entrepreneurship

4 Heritability of intelligence

5 Personality traits

6 Genetics

6.1 Genomics

6.2 Linkage and association studies

7 See also

8 References

9 Further reading

History of the debate

John Locke's An Essay Concerning Human Understanding (1690) is often cited as the foundational document of the "blank slate" view. Locke was criticizing René Descartes's claim of an innate idea of God universal to humanity. Locke's view was harshly criticized in his own time. Anthony Ashley-Cooper, 3rd Earl of Shaftesbury, complained that by denying the possibility of any innate ideas, Locke "threw all order and virtue out of the world", leading to total moral relativism. Locke's was not the predominant view in the 19th century, which on the contrary tended to focus on "instinct". Leda Cosmides and John Tooby noted that William James (1842–1910) argued that humans have more instincts than animals, and that greater freedom of action is the result of having more psychological instincts, not fewer.[19]

The question of "innate ideas" or "instincts" were of some importance in the discussion of free will in moral philosophy. In 18th-century philosophy, this was cast in terms of "innate ideas" establishing the presence of a universal virtue, prerequisite for objective morals. In the 20th century, this argument was in a way inverted, as some philosophers now argued that the evolutionary origins of human behavioral traits forces us to concede that there is no foundation for ethics (J. L. Mackie), while others treat ethics as a field in complete isolation from evolutionary considerations (Thomas Nagel).[20]

In the early 20th century, there was an increased interest in the role of the environment, as a reaction to the strong focus on pure heredity in the wake of the triumphal success of Darwin's theory of evolution.[21]

During this time, the social sciences developed as the project of studying the influence of culture in clean isolation from questions related to "biology". Franz Boas's The Mind of Primitive Man (1911) established a program that would dominate American anthropology for the next fifteen years. In this study he established that in any given population, biology, language, material and symbolic culture, are autonomous; that each is an equally important dimension of human nature, but that no one of these dimensions is reducible to another.

The tool of twin studies was developed as a research design intended to exclude all confounders based on inherited behavioral traits.[22] Such studies are designed to decompose the variability of a given trait in a given population into a genetic and an environmental component.

John B. Watson in the 1920s and 1930s established the school of purist behaviorism that would become dominant over the following decades. Watson is often said to have been convinced of the complete dominance of cultural influence over anything that heredity might contribute, based on the following quote which is frequently repeated without context:

"Give me a dozen healthy infants, well-formed, and my own specified world to bring them up in and I'll guarantee to take any one at random and train him to become any type of specialist I might select – doctor, lawyer, artist, merchant-chief and, yes, even beggar-man and thief, regardless of his talents, penchants, tendencies, abilities, vocations, and race of his ancestors. I am going beyond my facts and I admit it, but so have the advocates of the contrary and they have been doing it for many thousands of years" (Behaviorism, 1930, p. 82).

The last sentence of the above quote is frequently omitted, leading to confusion about Watson's position.

During the 1940s to 1960s, Ashley Montagu was a notable proponent of this purist form of behaviorism which allowed no contribution from heredity whatsoever:

"Man is man because he has no instincts, because everything he is and has become he has learned, acquired, from his culture ... with the exception of the instinctoid reactions in infants to sudden withdrawals of support and to sudden loud noises, the human being is entirely instinctless."[23]

In 1951, Calvin Hall[24] suggested that the dichotomy opposing nature to nurture is ultimately fruitless.

Robert Ardrey in the 1960s argued for innate attributes of human nature, especially concerning territoriality, in the widely read African Genesis (1961) and The Territorial Imperative. Desmond Morris in The Naked Ape (1967) expressed similar views. Organised opposition to Montagu's kind of purist "blank-slatism" began to pick up in the 1970s, notably led by E. O. Wilson (On Human Nature 1979). Twin studies established that there was, in many cases, a significant heritable component. These results did not in any way point to overwhelming contribution of heritable factors, with heritability typically ranging around 40% to 50%, so that the controversy may not be cast in terms of purist behaviorism vs. purist nativism. Rather, it was purist behaviorism which was gradually replaced by the now-predominant view that both kinds of factors usually contribute to a given trait, anecdotally phrased by Donald Hebb as an answer to the question "which, nature or nurture, contributes more to personality?" by asking in response, "Which contributes more to the area of a rectangle, its length or its width?"[25] In a comparable avenue of research, anthropologist Donald Brown in the 1980s surveyed hundreds of anthropological studies from around the world and collected a set of cultural universals. He identified approximately 150 such features, coming to the conclusion there is indeed a "universal human nature", and that these features point to what that universal human nature is.[26]

See also: Social determinism

At the height of the controversy, during the 1970s to 1980s, the debate was highly ideologised. In Not in Our Genes: Biology, Ideology and Human Nature (1984), Richard Lewontin, Steven Rose and Leon Kamin criticise "genetic determinism" from a Marxist framework, arguing that "Science is the ultimate legitimator of bourgeois ideology ... If biological determinism is a weapon in the struggle between classes, then the universities are weapons factories, and their teaching and research faculties are the engineers, designers, and production workers." The debate thus shifted away from whether heritable traits exist to whether it was politically or ethically permissible to admit their existence. The authors deny this, requesting that evolutionary inclinations be discarded in ethical and political discussions regardless of whether they exist or not.[27]

Heritability studies became much easier to perform, and hence much more numerous, with the advances of genetic studies during the 1990s. By the late 1990s, an overwhelming amount of evidence had accumulated that amounts to a refutation of the extreme forms of "blank-slatism" advocated by Watson or Montagu.

This revised state of affairs was summarized in books aimed at a popular audience from the late 1990s. In The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do (1998), Judith Rich Harris was heralded by Steven Pinker as a book that "will come to be seen as a turning point in the history of psychology".[28] but Harris was criticized for exaggerating the point of "parental upbringing seems to matter less than previously thought" to the implication that "parents do not matter".[29]

The situation as it presented itself by the end of the 20th century was summarized in The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature (2002) by Steven Pinker. The book became a best-seller, and was instrumental in bringing to the attention of a wider public the paradigm shift away from the behaviourist purism of the 1940s to 1970s that had taken place over the preceding decades. Pinker portrays the adherence to pure blank-slatism as an ideological dogma linked to two other dogmas found in the dominant view of human nature in the 20th century, which he termed "noble savage" (in the sense that people are born good and corrupted by bad influence) and "ghost in the machine" (in the sense that there is a human soul capable of moral choices completely detached from biology). Pinker argues that all three dogmas were held onto for an extended period even in the face of evidence because they were seen as desirable in the sense that if any human trait is purely conditioned by culture, any undesired trait (such as crime or aggression) may be engineered away by purely cultural (political means). Pinker focuses on reasons he assumes were responsible for unduly repressing evidence to the contrary, notably the fear of (imagined or projected) political or ideological consequences.[30]

Heritability estimates

Main article: Heritability

It is important to note that the term heritability refers only to the degree of genetic variation between people on a trait. It does not refer to the degree to which a trait of a particular individual is due to environmental or genetic factors. The traits of an individual are always a complex interweaving of both.[31] For an individual, even strongly genetically influenced, or "obligate" traits, such as eye color, assume the inputs of a typical environment during ontogenetic development (e.g., certain ranges of temperatures, oxygen levels, etc.).

In contrast, the "heritability index" statistically quantifies the extent to which variation between individuals on a trait is due to variation in the genes those individuals carry. In animals where breeding and environments can be controlled experimentally, heritability can be determined relatively easily. Such experiments would be unethical for human research. This problem can be overcome by finding existing populations of humans that reflect the experimental setting the researcher wishes to create.

One way to determine the contribution of genes and environment to a trait is to study twins. In one kind of study, identical twins reared apart are compared to randomly selected pairs of people. The twins share identical genes, but different family environments. Twins reared apart are not assigned at random to foster or adoptive parents. In another kind of twin study, identical twins reared together (who share family environment and genes) are compared to fraternal twins reared together (who also share family environment but only share half their genes). Another condition that permits the disassociation of genes and environment is adoption. In one kind of adoption study, biological siblings reared together (who share the same family environment and half their genes) are compared to adoptive siblings (who share their family environment but none of their genes).

In many cases, it has been found that genes make a substantial contribution, including psychological traits such as intelligence and personality.[32] Yet heritability may differ in other circumstances, for instance environmental deprivation. Examples of low, medium, and high heritability traits include:

Low heritability Medium heritability High heritability

Specific language Weight Blood type

Specific religion Religiosity Eye color

Twin and adoption studies have their methodological limits. For example, both are limited to the range of environments and genes which they sample. Almost all of these studies are conducted in Western, first-world countries, and therefore cannot be extrapolated globally to include poorer, non-western populations. Additionally, both types of studies depend on particular assumptions, such as the equal environments assumption in the case of twin studies, and the lack of pre-adoptive effects in the case of adoption studies.

Since the definition of "nature" in this context is tied to "heritability", the definition of "nurture" has necessarily become very wide, including any type of causality that is not heritable. The term has thus moved away from its original connotation of "cultural influences" to include all effects of the environment, including; indeed, a substantial source of environmental input to human nature may arise from stochastic variations in prenatal development and is thus in no sense of the term "cultural".[33][34]

Interaction of genes and environment

Main article: Gene–environment interaction

This section may stray from the topic of the article. Please help improve this section or discuss this issue on the talk page. (March 2015)

“ Many properties of the brain are genetically organized, and don't depend on information coming in from the senses. ”

— Steven Pinker

Heritability refers to the origins of differences between people. Individual development, even of highly heritable traits, such as eye color, depends on a range of environmental factors, from the other genes in the organism, to physical variables such as temperature, oxygen levels etc. during its development or ontogenesis.

The variability of trait can be meaningfully spoken of as being due in certain proportions to genetic differences ("nature"), or environments ("nurture"). For highly penetrant Mendelian genetic disorders such as Huntington's disease virtually all the incidence of the disease is due to genetic differences. Huntington's animal models live much longer or shorter lives depending on how they are cared for[citation needed].

At the other extreme, traits such as native language are environmentally determined: linguists have found that any child (if capable of learning a language at all) can learn any human language with equal facility.[35] With virtually all biological and psychological traits, however, genes and environment work in concert, communicating back and forth to create the individual.

At a molecular level, genes interact with signals from other genes and from the environment. While there are many thousands of single-gene-locus traits, so-called complex traits are due to the additive effects of many (often hundreds) of small gene effects. A good example of this is height, where variance appears to be spread across many hundreds of loci.[36]

Extreme genetic or environmental conditions can predominate in rare circumstances—if a child is born mute due to a genetic mutation, it will not learn to speak any language regardless of the environment; similarly, someone who is practically certain to eventually develop Huntington's disease according to their genotype may die in an unrelated accident (an environmental event) long before the disease will manifest itself.

The "two buckets" view of heritability.

More realistic "homogenous mudpie" view of heritability.

Steven Pinker likewise described several examples:[37][38]

concrete behavioral traits that patently depend on content provided by the home or culture—which language one speaks, which religion one practices, which political party one supports—are not heritable at all. But traits that reflect the underlying talents and temperaments—how proficient with language a person is, how religious, how liberal or conservative—are partially heritable.

When traits are determined by a complex interaction of genotype and environment it is possible to measure the heritability of a trait within a population. However, many non-scientists who encounter a report of a trait having a certain percentage heritability imagine non-interactional, additive contributions of genes and environment to the trait. As an analogy, some laypeople may think of the degree of a trait being made up of two "buckets," genes and environment, each able to hold a certain capacity of the trait. But even for intermediate heritabilities, a trait is always shaped by both genetic dispositions and the environments in which people develop, merely with greater and lesser plasticities associated with these heritability measures.

Heritability measures always refer to the degree of variation between individuals in a population. That is, as these statistics cannot be applied at the level of the individual, it would be incorrect to say that while the heritability index of personality is about 0.6, 60% of one's personality is obtained from one's parents and 40% from the environment. To help to understand this, imagine that all humans were genetic clones. The heritability index for all traits would be zero (all variability between clonal individuals must be due to environmental factors). And, contrary to erroneous interpretations of the heritability index, as societies become more egalitarian (everyone has more similar experiences) the heritability index goes up (as environments become more similar, variability between individuals is due more to genetic factors).

One should also take into account the fact that the variables of heritability and environmentality are not precise and vary within a chosen population and across cultures. It would be more accurate to state that the degree of heritability and environmentality is measured in its reference to a particular phenotype in a chosen group of a population in a given period of time. The accuracy of the calculations is further hindered by the number of coefficients taken into consideration, age being one such variable. The display of the influence of heritability and environmentality differs drastically across age groups: the older the studied age is, the more noticeable the heritability factor becomes, the younger the test subjects are, the more likely it is to show signs of strong influence of the environmental factors.

Some have pointed out that environmental inputs affect the expression of genes[16] (see the article on epigenetics). This is one explanation of how environment can influence the extent to which a genetic disposition will actually manifest.[citation needed] The interactions of genes with environment, called gene–environment interactions, are another component of the nature–nurture debate. A classic example of gene–environment interaction is the ability of a diet low in the amino acid phenylalanine to partially suppress the genetic disease phenylketonuria. Yet another complication to the nature–nurture debate is the existence of gene–environment correlations. These correlations indicate that individuals with certain genotypes are more likely to find themselves in certain environments. Thus, it appears that genes can shape (the selection or creation of) environments. Even using experiments like those described above, it can be very difficult to determine convincingly the relative contribution of genes and environment.

A study conducted by T. J. Bouchard, Jr. showed data that has been evidence for the importance of genes when testing middle-aged twins reared together and reared apart. The results shown have been important evidence against the importance of environment when determining, happiness, for example. In the Minnesota study of twins reared apart, it was actually found that there was higher correlation for monozygotic twins reared apart (0.52)than monozygotic twins reared together (0.44). Also, highlighting the importance of genes, these correlations found much higher correlation among monozygotic than dizygotic twins that had a correlation of 0.08 when reared together and −0.02 when reared apart.[39]

Social pre-wiring

Question book-new.svg

This section relies largely or entirely upon a single source. Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page. Please help improve this article by introducing citations to additional sources.

Find sources: "Nature versus nurture" – news · newspapers · books · scholar · JSTOR (December 2017) (Learn how and when to remove this template message)

The social pre-wiring hypothesis refers to the ontogeny of social interaction. Also informally referred to as, "wired to be social." The theory questions whether there is a propensity to socially oriented action already present before birth. Research in the theory concludes that newborns are born into the world with a unique genetic wiring to be social.[40]

Circumstantial evidence supporting the social pre-wiring hypothesis can be revealed when examining newborns' behavior. Newborns, not even hours after birth, have been found to display a preparedness for social interaction. This preparedness is expressed in ways such as their imitation of facial gestures. This observed behavior cannot be contributed to any current form of socialization or social construction. Rather, newborns most likely inherit to some extent social behavior and identity through genetics.[40]

Principal evidence of this theory is uncovered by examining twin pregnancies. The main argument is, if there are social behaviors that are inherited and developed before birth, then one should expect twin foetuses to engage in some form of social interaction before they are born. Thus, ten foetuses were analyzed over a period of time using ultrasound techniques. Using kinematic analysis, the results of the experiment were that the twin foetuses would interact with each other for longer periods and more often as the pregnancies went on. Researchers were able to conclude that the performance of movements between the co-twins were not accidental but specifically aimed.[40]

The social pre-wiring hypothesis was proved correct, "The central advance of this study is the demonstration that 'social actions' are already performed in the second trimester of gestation. Starting from the 14th week of gestation twin foetuses plan and execute movements specifically aimed at the co-twin. These findings force us to predate the emergence of social behavior: when the context enables it, as in the case of twin foetuses, other-directed actions are not only possible but predominant over self-directed actions.".[40]

Obligate vs. facultative adaptations

Traits may be considered to be adaptations (such as the umbilical cord), byproducts of adaptations (the belly button) or due to random variation (convex or concave belly button shape).[41] An alternative to contrasting nature and nurture focuses on "obligate vs. facultative" adaptations.[41] Adaptations may be generally more obligate (robust in the face of typical environmental variation) or more facultative (sensitive to typical environmental variation). For example, the rewarding sweet taste of sugar and the pain of bodily injury are obligate psychological adaptations—typical environmental variability during development does not much affect their operation.[42] On the other hand, facultative adaptations are somewhat like "if-then" statements.[43] An example of a facultative psychological adaptation may be adult attachment style. The attachment style of adults, (for example, a "secure attachment style," the propensity to develop close, trusting bonds with others) is proposed to be conditional on whether an individual's early childhood caregivers could be trusted to provide reliable assistance and attention. An example of a facultative physiological adaptation is tanning of skin on exposure to sunlight (to prevent skin damage). Facultative social adaptation have also been proposed. For example, whether a society is warlike or peaceful has been proposed to be conditional on how much collective threat that society is experiencing [44].

Advanced techniques

Quantitative studies of heritable traits throw light on the question.

Developmental genetic analysis examines the effects of genes over the course of a human lifespan. Early studies of intelligence, which mostly examined young children, found that heritability measured 40–50%. Subsequent developmental genetic analyses found that variance attributable to additive environmental effects is less apparent in older individuals,[45][46][47] with estimated heritability of IQ increasing in adulthood.

Multivariate genetic analysis examines the genetic contribution to several traits that vary together. For example, multivariate genetic analysis has demonstrated that the genetic determinants of all specific cognitive abilities (e.g., memory, spatial reasoning, processing speed) overlap greatly, such that the genes associated with any specific cognitive ability will affect all others. Similarly, multivariate genetic analysis has found that genes that affect scholastic achievement completely overlap with the genes that affect cognitive ability.

Extremes analysis examines the link between normal and pathological traits. For example, it is hypothesized that a given behavioral disorder may represent an extreme of a continuous distribution of a normal behavior and hence an extreme of a continuous distribution of genetic and environmental variation. Depression, phobias, and reading disabilities have been examined in this context.

For a few highly heritable traits, studies have identified loci associated with variance in that trait, for instance in some individuals with schizophrenia.[48]

Entrepreneurship

Through studies of identical twins separated at birth, one-third of their creative thinking abilities come from genetics and two-thirds come from learning.[49] Research suggests that between 37 and 42 percent of the explained variance can be attributed to genetic factors.[50] The learning primarily comes in the form of human capital transfers of entrepreneurial skills through parental role modeling.[51] Other findings agree that the key to innovative entrepreneurial success comes from environmental factors and working “10,000 hours” to gain mastery in entrepreneurial skills.[52]

Heritability of intelligence

Main article: Heritability of IQ

Evidence from behavioral genetic research suggests that family environmental factors may have an effect upon childhood IQ, accounting for up to a quarter of the variance. The American Psychological Association's report "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns" (1995) states that there is no doubt that normal child development requires a certain minimum level of responsible care. Here, environment is playing a role in what is believed to be fully genetic (intelligence) but it was found that severely deprived, neglectful, or abusive environments have highly negative effects on many aspects of children's intellect development. Beyond that minimum, however, the role of family experience is in serious dispute. On the other hand, by late adolescence this correlation disappears, such that adoptive siblings no longer have similar IQ scores.[53]

Moreover, adoption studies indicate that, by adulthood, adoptive siblings are no more similar in IQ than strangers (IQ correlation near zero), while full siblings show an IQ correlation of 0.6. Twin studies reinforce this pattern: monozygotic (identical) twins raised separately are highly similar in IQ (0.74), more so than dizygotic (fraternal) twins raised together (0.6) and much more than adoptive siblings (~0.0).[54] Recent adoption studies also found that supportive parents can have a positive effect on the development of their children.[55]

Personality traits

Main article: Personality psychology § Genetic basis of personality

Personality is a frequently cited example of a heritable trait that has been studied in twins and adoptees using behavioral genetic study designs. The most famous categorical organization of heritable personality traits were defined in the 1970s by two research teams led by Paul Costa & Robert R. McCrae and Warren Norman & Lewis Goldberg in which they had people rate their personalities on 1000+ dimensions they then narrowed these down into "The Big Five" factors of personality—openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism. The close genetic relationship between positive personality traits and, for example, our happiness traits are the mirror images of comorbidity in psychopathology. These personality factors were consistent across cultures, and many studies have also tested the heritability of these traits.

Identical twins reared apart are far more similar in personality than randomly selected pairs of people. Likewise, identical twins are more similar than fraternal twins. Also, biological siblings are more similar in personality than adoptive siblings. Each observation suggests that personality is heritable to a certain extent. A supporting article had focused on the heritability of personality (which is estimated to be around 50% for subjective well-being) in which a study was conducted using a representative sample of 973 twin pairs to test the heritable differences in subjective well-being which were found to be fully accounted for by the genetic model of the Five-Factor Model’s personality domains.[56] However, these same study designs allow for the examination of environment as well as genes.

Adoption studies also directly measure the strength of shared family effects. Adopted siblings share only family environment. Most adoption studies indicate that by adulthood the personalities of adopted siblings are little or no more similar than random pairs of strangers. This would mean that shared family effects on personality are zero by adulthood.

In the case of personality traits, non-shared environmental effects are often found to out-weigh shared environmental effects. That is, environmental effects that are typically thought to be life-shaping (such as family life) may have less of an impact than non-shared effects, which are harder to identify. One possible source of non-shared effects is the environment of pre-natal development. Random variations in the genetic program of development may be a substantial source of non-shared environment. These results suggest that "nurture" may not be the predominant factor in "environment". Environment and our situations, do in fact impact our lives, but not the way in which we would typically react to these environmental factors. We are preset with personality traits that are the basis for how we would react to situations. An example would be how extraverted prisoners become less happy than introverted prisoners and would react to their incarceration more negatively due to their preset extraverted personality.[31]:Ch 19 Behavioral genes are somewhat proven to exist when we take a look at fraternal twins. When fraternal twins are reared apart, they show the same similarities in behavior and response as if they have been reared together.[57]

Genetics

Genomics

Main article: Genomics

The relationship between personality and people's own well-being is influenced and mediated by genes (Weiss, Bates, & Luciano, 2008). There has been found to be a stable set point for happiness that is characteristic of the individual (largely determined by the individual's genes). Happiness fluctuates around that setpoint (again, genetically determined) based on whether good things or bad things are happening to us ("nurture"), but only fluctuates in small magnitude in a normal human. The midpoint of these fluctuations is determined by the "great genetic lottery" that people are born with, which leads them to conclude that how happy they may feel at the moment or over time is simply due to the luck of the draw, or gene. This fluctuation was also not due to educational attainment, which only accounted for less than 2% of the variance in well-being for women, and less than 1% of the variance for men.[39]

They consider that the individualities measured together with personality tests remain steady throughout an individual’s lifespan. They further believe that human beings may refine their forms or personality but can never change them entirely. Darwin's Theory of Evolution steered naturalists such as George Williams and William Hamilton to the concept of personality evolution. They suggested that physical organs and also personality is a product of natural selection.[58]

With the advent of genomic sequencing, it has become possible to search for and identify specific gene polymorphisms that affect traits such as IQ and personality. These techniques work by tracking the association of differences in a trait of interest with differences in specific molecular markers or functional variants. An example of a visible human trait for which the precise genetic basis of differences are relatively well known is eye color. For traits with many genes affecting the outcome, a smaller portion of the variance is currently understood: For instance for height known gene variants account for around 5–10% of height variance at present.[citation needed] When discussing the significant role of genetic heritability in relation to one's level of happiness, it has been found that from 44% to 52% of the variance in one's well-being is associated with genetic variation. Based on the retest of smaller samples of twins studies after 4,5, and 10 years, it is estimated that the heritability of the genetic stable component of subjective well-being approaches 80%.[39] Other studies that have found that genes are a large influence in the variance found in happiness measures, exactly around 35–50%.[59][60][61][62]

In contrast to views developed in 1960s that gender identity is primarily learned (which led to policy-based surgical sex changed in children such as David Reimer), genomics has provided solid evidence that both sex and gender identities are primarily influenced by genes:

It is now clear that genes are vastly more influential than virtually any other force in shaping sex identity and gender identity…[T]he growing consensus in medicine is that…children should be assigned to their chromosomal (i.e., genetic) sex regardless of anatomical variations and differences—with the option of switching, if desired, later in life.

— Siddhartha Mukherjee, The Gene: An Intimate History

Linkage and association studies

In their attempts to locate the genes responsible for configuring certain phenotypes, researches resort to two different techniques. Linkage study facilitates the process of determining a specific location in which a gene of interest is located. This methodology is applied only among individuals that are related and does not serve to pinpoint specific genes. It does, however, narrow down the area of search, making it easier to locate one or several genes in the genome which constitute a specific trait.

Association studies, on the other hand, are more hypothetic and seek to verify whether a particular genetic variable really influences the phenotype of interest. In association studies it is more common to use case-control approach, comparing the subject with relatively higher or lower hereditary determinants with the control subject.

See also

Behavioral genetics

Behavioral epigenetics

Dual inheritance theory

Nature–culture divide

Niche picking

Social determinism

Social constructionism

Sociobiology

Structure and agency

Identical Strangers

Three Identical Strangers .

..

En psicología, la expresión innato o adquirido hace referencia al antiguo debate respecto a si las características físicas, las enfermedades orgánicas, los trastornos emocionales o incluso el comportamiento de los individuos tienen un origen y si éste es social o ambiental, es decir, si se derivan de la educación, o de la convivencia familiar.

El debate tenía que ver, pues, con la importancia relativa de las cualidades innatas de un individuo (el entonces llamado nativismo psicológico o innatismo) y las experiencias personales (la crianza, es decir, el empirismo o conductismo) en la determinación o causa de las diferencias individuales en los rasgos físicos y conductuales.

La frase fue acuñada por el científico británico de la era victoriana Francis Galton,1​2​3​ inspirado en la obra El origen de las especies, de su primo Charles Darwin, para determinar la influencia de la herencia y del medio ambiente sobre las carreras sociales.4​ El concepto que se halla implícito en esta expresión ha sido muy criticado3​4​ debido a su simplificación binaria de dos parámetros que en realidad se hallan entrelazados en una red compleja.

La postura de que los seres humanos adquieren todos o casi todos sus rasgos conductuales a partir de la crianza decía que somos una tabula rasa ("pizarra en blanco"). Este asunto se consideraba antes una división apropiada de las influencias ambientales, pero desde que se sabe que el funcionamiento de ambos tipos de factores (ambientales y hereditarios) interactúan en el desarrollo de un individuo muchos especialistas de las ciencias del comportamiento consideran, en la actualidad, que el debate ya no tiene sentido y que representa un periodo ya superado en la historia del conocimiento.5​6​7​8​ Se dice que en una ocasión un periodista le preguntó al psicólogo Donald Hebb: "¿Qué contribuye más a la personalidad: la herencia o el ambiente?", y que él contestó con una pregunta: "¿Qué contribuye más al área de un rectángulo: su largo o su ancho?"9​10​11​12​ En otras palabras, la idea de que o bien la naturaleza (lo genético, lo hereditario) o bien la crianza (lo aprendido, la educación, el medio ambiente familiar y social) determina el comportamiento es una especie de falacia de una sola causa.

En las ciencias sociales este debate puede compararse con el debate structure versus agency, es decir, socialización contra autonomía individual. Para saber más sobre la relación entre el debate innato-adquirido y el lenguaje y otros aspectos universales del ser humano, véase también nativismo psicológico.

Índice

1 Enfoque científico

2 Debate sobre el CI

3 Definición de términos

4 Las dificultades para estimar la capacidad hereditaria

5 Determinismo biológico

6 Fenocopias

7 Estudio de gemelos

8 Técnicas de estudio

9 Otras dificultades

10 Mitos acerca de la identidad

11 Véase también

12 Referencias

13 Bibliografía

Enfoque científico

Para dirimir los efectos de los genes y del ambiente, los genetistas conductuales llevan a cabo estudios sobre la adopción y sobre los gemelos. Los genetistas conductuales no suelen usar el término adquirido para explicar el porcentaje de la varianza correspondiente a un rasgo específico (por ejemplo, el cociente intelectual (CI) o los cinco rasgos de la personalidad) que pueden atribuirse a efectos del medio ambiente. Por el contrario, se distinguen dos tipos distintos de efectos ambientales: factores familiares compartidos (los compartidos por los hermanos, lo que los hace más similares) y los factores no compartidos (los que afectan únicamente a cada uno de los individuos, lo que los hace distintos). A fin de expresar el porcentaje de varianza que se debe al componente "adquirido", los genetistas conductuales generalmente hacen referencia a la heredabilidad de un rasgo.

Con respecto a los cinco grandes rasgos de la personalidad y al CI de los adultos en la población general en los Estados Unidos, el porcentaje de la varianza general que puede atribuirse a efectos familiares compartidos por lo general no se toma en cuenta.13​ Por otra parte, se cree que la mayor parte de los rasgos pueden heredarse al menos parcialmente. En este contexto, se cree que el componente "heredado" de la varianza es más importante que el que se atribuye a la influencia de la crianza familiar.

En su libro The Nurture Assumption, nominado al Premio Pulitzer, Judith Harris afirma que lo adquirido, definido tradicionalmente desde el punto de vista de la crianza familiar, no explica de manera eficaz la varianza de la mayor parte de los rasgos (como, por ejemplo, el CI adulto y los cinco grandes rasgos de la personalidad) en la población general de los Estados Unidos. Por el contrario, ella sugiere que son más importantes los grupos de pares o una serie de factores ambientales aleatorios (es decir, todos aquellos que son independientes de la crianza familiar) que los efectos ambientales familiares.14​15​

Solamente en contados casos es correcto decir que un rasgo es debido casi en su totalidad a la naturaleza. En el caso de la mayor de las enfermedades identificadas como genéticas, tales como el mal de Huntington, hay una correlación aproximada del 99,9 % entre poseer el gen identificado y poseer el mal y una correlación similar en no poseer ninguno de las dos. Por el contrario, rasgos como el lenguaje nativo son enteramente determinados: los lingüistas han encontrado que cualquier niño (con capacidad suficiente de aprender un lenguaje cualquiera) puede aprender cualquier lenguaje humano con igual facilidad. De todos modos, con virtualmente todos los rasgos psicológicos, hay una mezcla intermedia de innato y adquirido, y las opiniones acerca de la importancia relativa de cada una a menudo varían ampliamente.

Algunos ejemplos de rasgos ambientales, interactivos y genéticos:

Predominantemente ambientales Interactivos Predominantemente genéticos

Lenguaje Altura Tipo de sangre

Religión Peso Color de ojos

Color de piel

Coeficiente de inteligencia

Steven Pinker (2004) ha descrito varios ejemplos:

Los rasgos concretos de comportamiento que patentemente dependen del contenido provisto en la casa o la cultura -qué lenguaje se habla, qué religión se practica, qué partido político se fomenta- no son hereditarios, pero aquellos rasgos que reflejan ciertos talentos y temperamentos subyacentes – qué tan avanzada está una lengua en cada persona, qué tan devota, liberal o conservadora es- son parcialmente hereditarios.

Cuando los rasgos son determinados por una interacción compleja de genotipos y medio ambiente, resulta posible medir la capacidad hereditaria de un rasgo en una determinada población de individuos. De todos modos, muchos observadores, al encontrar un rasgo que tiene un cierto porcentaje de capacidad hereditaria, imaginan contribuciones aditivas no-interactivas de genes y medio ambiente en dichos rasgos; es como si se pensara que el grado de un rasgo está compuesto por dos "baldes", genes y medio ambiente, cada uno capaz de contener cierta capacidad para el rasgo. Pero aun para una capacidad hereditaria intermedia, un rasgo está siempre modificado por ambas variables: la genética y el medio ambiente en que la gente se desarrolla, meramente con mayor o menor plasticidad asociada con estas medidas de capacidad hereditaria.

Debate sobre el CI

Artículo principal: Heredabilidad del CI

La evidencia sugiere que factores de ambiente familiar pueden tener efecto en el desarrollo del IQ durante la niñez, representando hasta una cuarta parte de la varianza. Más adelante, en la adolescencia, esta relación desaparece de manera que, por ejemplo, los hermanos adoptados se diferencian en el coeficiente intelectual al igual que extraños.16​

Más aún, estudios de adopción indican que en la edad adulta la similitud en el coeficiente intelectual de los hermanos adoptados es prácticamente cero, cuando en los hermanos plenos se presenta una correlación de 0.6. En los gemelos se refuerza el patrón: gemelos monocigóticos (idénticos) criados separadamente presentan un CI muy similar con una correlación de 0.74, más que los gemelos dicigóticos criados juntos (0.6).17​

En resumen, el CI parece ser casi totalmente genético.[cita requerida]

Definición de términos

• Adquirido: Este término se utiliza para referirse a cualquier factor ambiental (no genético, no hereditario). Su definición incluye las influencias en el desarrollo provenientes de experiencias prenatales paternales o maternales, familiares o de pares, y se extiende a influencias tales como los medios de comunicación, las influencias del mercado y las condiciones socioeconómicas.

• Innato: Se refiere a factores genéticos, de nacimiento o hereditarios, es decir, que no dependen de la experiencia individual.

Las dificultades para estimar la capacidad hereditaria

El pensamiento actual en biología desacredita la noción de que los genes por sí solos pueden determinar un rasgo, debido a que éstos no son nunca suficientes de forma aislada. A nivel molecular, el ADN interacciona en forma compleja con señales de otros genes y del medio ambiente.

(Véase exogenética.)

Entonces, tenemos:

Efecto del ambiente externo

-Temperatura: Es de esperar que en el desarrollo de los organismos vivos se experimenten distintos efectos con el cambio de temperatura, produciendo ésta a veces efectos bastante dramáticos.

Ejemplo: color blanco de las flores de Oenothera cuando la temperatura es mayor a 86 °F, oscurecimiento del pelaje de las extremidades de algunos mamíferos por existir una temperatura baja en la zona donde se encuentra (cosa que cambia si se traslada a un lugar de atmósfera cálida), etc.

Además, podría decirse que el efecto de la interacción gen-ambiente puede invertirse por simples sustituciones génicas, a pesar de permanecer la interacción.

-Luz: En prácticamente todas las plantas es esencial para el crecimiento y desarrollo de la expresión completa del genotipo, ya que una insuficiencia de ésta provoca la falta de producción de clorofila (dando plantas albinas). Otro efecto generado por la luz, por ejemplo, es la presencia de pecas en la cara cuando individuos de cierto genotipo se exponen al sol.

-Nutrición: A pesar de ser la principal fuente de energía, el tipo y cantidad de nutrientes obtenidos de los alimentos que requieren los organismos varía entre unos y otros. Así, las posibles deficiencias metabólicas son más fáciles de detectar en los que tienen necesidades nutritivas sencillas, pudiéndose añadir en su caso a su dieta nutrientes extras para que sean capaces de sintetizar compuestos específicos.

También, puede verse que cambios sencillos en la dieta pueden llevar diversas consecuencias, como por ejemplo la eliminación de grasa amarilla en los conejos si se les quitan las verduras.

-Relaciones maternas: Pueden producirse interacciones ambientales adicionales entre el feto y el ambiente externo cuando las relaciones corporales directas entre el progenitor y la progenie se extienden más allá de la fecundación (mamíferos), apareciendo por ejemplo problemas en la supervivencia de determinados genotipos a causa de las incompatibilidades de grupos sanguíneos entre madre e hijo (además de poder resultar desventajosos ciertos caracteres que se consideran normales, en ambientes maternos especiales).

Efectos del medio interno

-Edad: El proceso del tiempo puede considerarse que en la mayoría de los organismos comienza en la fecundación, existiendo intervalos de edad sucesivos con numerosos cambios fenotípicos.

Ejemplo: cambios en el color del pelo de los mamíferos, o en el de plumas de gallinas.

-Sexo: Los fenotipos que acompañan a las diferencias sexuales son por lo general evidentes en muchas características físicas asociadas con las funciones reproductoras y con la conducta especializada de cada sexo. Así, hay genes ligados al sexo que dan caracteres limitados e influenciados por ellos.

-Sustratos: Según los materiales que se hallan presentes en el interior de los organismos, ocurren en ellos distintos tipos de reacciones. Estos son en muchos casos sintetizados por acciones metabólicas del organismo y, con frecuencia, su presencia o ausencia puede remontarse a un control genético.

Ejemplo: una dieta reducida en el aminoácido fenilalanina tiene la capacidad de suprimir la condición genética fenilcetonuria; enfermedad hereditaria humana que se diagnostica por la presencia en la orina de ácido fenilpirúvico.

Determinismo biológico

A nivel de individuos los genes particulares influencian el desarrollo de un cierto rasgo en el contexto de un medio ambiente particular. De allí, una medida del grado en que un rasgo es influenciado por los genes versus el medio ambiente dependerá del ambiente particular y de los genes examinados. En muchos casos se ha encontrado que los genes pueden tener una influencia substancial en el desarrollo de las características personales, incluyendo rasgos psicológicos tales como inteligencia y personalidad. Por ello algunos se preguntan si esto implica que los genes determinan quiénes somos, siendo el Determinismo biológico la tesis que propone esta cuestión.

Pocos son los científicos que sostienen esta opinión, pero muchos son acusados de hacerlo. Otros han indicado que las premisas del debate innato o adquirido parecen negar el principio del libre albedrío. Más específicamente, si todos nuestros rasgos están determinados por nuestros genes, por el medio ambiente que nos rodea por el azar o por alguna combinación de todas estas causas actuando simultáneamente, queda muy poca opción para el libre albedrío. En cualquier caso esta línea de razonamiento sugiere que el debate innato o adquirido tiende a exagerar el grado en que el comportamiento humano individual puede ser predicho basado en el conocimiento de genética y medio ambiente. Deberíamos notar que la biología determina nuestras habilidades pero el libre albedrío determina que hacemos con nuestras habilidades.

Interés especial en los estudios de gemelos de Newman y sus colaboradores se muestra en los coeficientes de inteligencia Stanford-Binet. Aunque los tests IQ presentan numerosos deficientes (el tamaño de la familia, el efecto cultural, el primer ambiente y la madre y los efectos de la dieta son factores ambientales determinantes de sus componentes, por ejemplo), ofrecen atributos mentales importantes: mediante su empleo, en resumen se puede decir que la separación del efecto ambiental de la herencia en lo que se refiere a la inteligencia es una tarea difícil, ya que este carácter es el resultado de una interacción continua entre los estímulos externos al organismo y las respuestas que éste es capaz de dar. Además, las respuestas de comportamiento en sí dependen mucho de los ambientes previos, algunos de los cuales son influidos o creados por el propio organismo. Es decir, a menudo se incorpora el “ambiente” psicológicamente al individuo, de manera que sus respuestas son restringidas sucesivamente por estas interacciones que han tenido lugar en el pasado. Cuando se está tratando un proceso histórico de esta clase, ciertamente no queda claro qué porcentaje de una respuesta psicológica puede ser imputado a la “naturaleza” y a la “educación”.

Con todo esto, se puede llegar a dos conceptos sencillos e importantes: que genotipos distintos pueden reaccionar de forma distinta en el mismo ambiente, y que el mismo genotipo puede comportarse de maneras diferentes en distintos ambientes: Norma de reacción.

Fenocopias

Éstos son los individuos en los que efectos de muchos genes pueden verse modificados por la fuerza de los cambios ambientales; algunos de estos cambios son capaces de modificar el desarrollo de un organismo de modo que su fenotipo simule el efecto de un gen en particular, aunque dicho efecto no sea heredado.

Esto ocurre, por ejemplo, en los diabéticos; dependen de la insulina, y son fenocopias de individuos normales en el sentido de que el ambiente químico evita el efecto de la enfermedad.

Estudio de gemelos

Una manera de determinar el aporte de los genes y del medio ambiente en la presencia de un rasgo es estudiando los casos de hermanos gemelos y mellizos. Un tipo de estudio consiste en comparar gemelos criados separadamente con cualquier par de individuos seleccionados aleatoriamente en una población, compartiendo en este caso los gemelos los mismos genes pero no el medio ambiente. En otro caso se toman gemelos criados juntos (compartiendo así genes y medio ambiente) y se comparan con mellizos criados juntos (estos comparten la mitad de sus genes y el medio ambiente) Otra situación que permite la disociación de la influencia de genes y medio ambiente es la adopción.

En el caso de estudio de adopción hermanos biológicos criados juntos (compartiendo la mitad de sus genes y el medio ambiente) son comparados con hermanos adoptivos (quienes sólo comparten el medio ambiente dado por la familia y no así los genes);los hermanos gemelos son mucho más similares en su personalidad que pares de individuos seleccionados aleatoriamente entre la población. De una manera similar los gemelos son más similares que los mellizos.

Finalmente los hermanos biológicos son más similares que los hermanos adoptivos. Estas observaciones sugieren que la personalidad es hereditaria hasta un determinado punto.

Estos mismos métodos de estudio pueden ser utilizados tanto para determinar la influencia del medio ambiente como para la de los genes. Típicamente se distinguen dos clases de efectos ambientales, efectos de la familia compartida (aquellos compartidos por los hermanos haciéndolos más similares) y efectos no compartidos (aquellos que afectan solamente al individuo haciendo a los hermanos diferentes). Los gemelos criados juntos, a pesar de ser genéticamente idénticos y compartir el mismo ambiente familiar no poseen idénticas personalidades. Estas diferencias son causadas exclusivamente (por definición) por efectos ambientales no compartidos.

Al contrario de lo esperado, algunos estudios de adopción indican que al llegar a ser adultos las personalidades de niños adoptados no son más similares que pares de extraños escogidos al azar.

Esto significaría que el efecto de la familia compartida sobre la personalidad disminuye a cero al llegar a la adultez.

Como en el caso de la personalidad, los efectos ambientales no compartidos sobrepasan los compartidos. De esta manera ciertos efectos ambientales a los que típicamente se atribuyen un efecto formativo (tales como la vida familiar) pueden llegar a tener menor impacto que los no compartidos que son más difíciles de identificar. Una fuente posible de efectos no compartidos es el ambiente de desarrollo prenatal. Variaciones aleatorias en el programa genético de desarrollo pueden ser una fuente substancial de efectos no compartidos.

Estos resultados sugieren que la educación y cuidado pueden no ser un factor predominante en el medio ambiente.

Algunos han señalado que las razones ambientales afectan la expresión de los genes. Esta es una explicación de cómo el medio ambiente puede influenciar la medida en que una disposición genética se manifiesta. Las interacciones de los genes con el medio ambiente llamadas interacciones genético-ambientales son otro aspecto del debate innato o adquirido: una complicación más es la existencia de correlaciones genético-ambientales, las cuales indican que individuos que poseen ciertos genotipos son más propensos a encontrarse en determinados medio ambientes. Así parecería que los genes pueden influenciar la selección o creación de medio ambientes, haciendo muy difícil determinar convincentemente la contribución relativa de genes y medio ambiente en los rasgos de un individuo.

Técnicas de estudio

Para ver los papeles relativos que tienen el ambiente y la herencia, se pueden emplear diversas técnicas:

*Concordancia-discordancia: Una de las formas en que pueden medirse las similitudes o diferencias fenotípicas de un carácter en los gemelos consiste simplemente en anotar si éste está presente o no en uno o ambos miembros. Así, si ambos o ninguno lo tienen serían concordantes (fenotípicamente similares)…pero si sólo uno lo tiene, serían discordantes (fenotípicamente disimilares).

*Estudios cuantitativos de los rasgos hereditarios: Esto ha sido expandido con el desarrollo de nuevas técnicas.

a) El análisis de desarrollo genético examina el efecto de los genes sobre el transcurso de una vida humana.

-los primitivos estudios de inteligencia que examinaban mayormente niños de edad temprana, encontraba valores de capacidad hereditaria del 40 al 50 %. Posteriores análisis de desarrollo genético han encontrado que la contribución genética hacia la inteligencia aumenta a medida que se progresa en la vida, alcanzando un 80 % en la edad adulta.

b) El análisis genético multivariable examina la contribución genética hacia varios rasgos que varían simultáneamente.

-este análisis ha demostrado que los determinantes genéticos de todas las habilidades cognitivas (memoria, razonamiento espacial, velocidad de procesamiento) se superponen en gran medida, de tal manera que los genes asociados con cualquier habilidad cognitiva específica afectara las otras. También, ha encontrado que los genes que afectan el desenvolvimiento escolar se superponen completamente con los genes que afectan la capacidad cognitiva.

c) El análisis de extremos examina la conexión entre los rasgos normales y los patológicos. Un ejemplo es la hipótesis que un desorden de comportamiento puede representar un extremo de un una distribución continua de un comportamiento normal y de allí un extremo de una distribución continua de variaciones genéticas y ambientales. La depresión, la fobia y los problemas de lectura han sido examinados en este contexto. Para rasgos altamente hereditarios es posible ahora buscar los genes individuales que contribuyen a la variación de tal rasgo.

Otras dificultades

Algunos observadores creen que la ciencia moderna tiende a dar demasiado peso a la posición del debate que favorece a lo innato, en parte debido a la conciencia social. Históricamente mucho de lo dicho en este debate ha tenido un tinte racista, y la política eugenística (la noción de raza como una verdad científica) ha sido a menudo un requisito en varias instancias de este debate.

En el pasado, la herencia fue usada a menudo como una justificación 'científica' para varias formas de discriminación y opresión racial y clasista.

Una crítica hacia los argumentos morales en contra de la postura por lo innato en el debate es que estos argumentos cruzan la barrera de lo que es y lo que debería ser. Es decir, aplican valores a los hechos (cuestionándose la existencia real de los rasgos que se tratan de medir en algunos casos).

Mitos acerca de la identidad

Dentro de los debates acerca de la clonación, el pensamiento actual descarta la posibilidad de que el clon de cualquiera pudiese llegar a ser el mismo individuo debido a la variación ambiental.

Ello se apoya, por ejemplo, en que al igual que los clones los gemelos son genéticamente idénticos y a pesar de compartir el mismo ambiente familiar no demuestran ser idénticos en personalidad u otros rasgos.

Véase también

adquisición del lenguaje

darwinismo social

eugenesia

evolución biológica

evolución cultural

lamarquismo

lista de prejuicios cognitivos

modelo de diátesis-estrés

naturaleza humana

David Reimer

teoría de los sistemas de desarrollo

tabula rasa.

. .

Dezbaterea pe tema "innăscut vs dobândit" ("născut vs făcut") implică intrebarea dacă comportamentul uman este determinat de mediul prenatal sau cel din timpul vieții unei persoane sau este determinat de genele respectivei persoane. Expresia aliterativă „născut vs făcut”, în engleză a fost folosită încă din perioada elisabetană, cu rădăcini ce pătrund până prin perioada francezei medievale.

¡¡¡ Traducción en curso!!!

Traducere in curs.