To what extent do added stimuli impact the level of focus in elementary aged students in online classes in western Washington as reported by parents or guardians?
Maya Ford
Fordmay000@stu.sumnersd.org
Class of 2021
There is a problem with children focusing in the online learning platform. Although many students had no problem focusing in a typical classroom, online learning is proving to be difficult and parents are struggling to find a solution for their child. This problem is impacting students because they are struggling to focus, parents because they are struggling to find a solution for their child and teachers because they are struggling to teach students who are not engaged and ready to learn. A possible cause of the focusing problem could be a lack of stimuli. A study which investigates stimulus impact on focus in elementary students in the online classroom could remedy this situation.
The COVID-19 pandemic forced students to move to online school in a matter of a few weeks. This left teachers, students, parents, and administration ill-prepared to teach online. It was proving difficult for children to focus in their new learning environment, as they had to relearn signals from their sensory system. Added stimuli are able to regulate energy levels and connect the brain to the sensory system.
If students level of focus is tested with varying stimulus (music, gum and a hand fidget toy), then the hand fidget toy will prove to be the most effective at increasing focus in students while music will show to decrease focus because studies have shown that hand fidget toys are effective at increasing focus in elementary aged children in a normal classroom setting and music only increases focus in a small percentage of people.
Western Washington: West of the Cascade Mountains in Washington State
Elementary Students: Students enrolled in an elementary or primary school, born Sept. 1 2008-Sept.1 2015
Online learning: students who participate in classes online for 50% or more of their school week.
Loses focus: playing with animals; being on phone (except for teacher assigns tasks using a phone), playing with toys; folding, cutting, drawing or coloring papers (except when directed by the teacher); staring at an object that is not school-related for more than ten seconds; excessive bathroom usage (more than 1 time in the ½ hour); excessive snacking or eating (more than 1 time in the ½ hour); talking to people outside of the classroom (people in the house or students in other classrooms for longer than 10 seconds) in the last ½ hour of their school day
Experimental, descriptive survey: Pre-test, post-test only design, each student will participate in each section of the experiment, creating their own data set. Each students data will be compared only to them, rather than comparing data across students.
The first day, each student will go about their school day as normal, with each parent/guardian recording how many times their student loses focus in the last half hour of their school day. On the second day, the student will be provided with a hand fidget toy, and once again parents will record how many times their child loses focus in the last half hour of their school day. The third day the child will chew on a piece of mint gum in the last half hour of their school day and parents will once again record how many times their student loses focus. On the fourth and final day, the child will listen to mozart music for the last half hour of the school day and parents/guardians will record how many times their child loses focus.
Due to COVID-19, I, the researcher, cannot collect data in person, and rather must rely on parents to accurately record and report data.
This research project will find, communicate with participants and report data online, which limits participants to those with internet access.
Due to limitations on in-person contact, eye tracking software could not be used to measure focus.
Overall, added stimuli increased focusing levels in elementary children during online classes. Children with a history of using chewing gum saw a decrease in their focusing levels during the mint chewing gum trial. Children with little to no history of using chewing gum saw an increase in their focusing levels during the mint chewing gum trial. Children with a history of listening to classical music while performing a task saw an increase in focusing levels during the classical music trial. Children without a history of listening to classical music while performing a task saw a decrease in their focusing levels during the classical music trial. No matter the child's history with hand fidget tools, hand fidget tools showed to increase focusing levels in all children.
The conclusions drawn in this study may encourage parents/guardians to incorporate added stimuli into their child’s online learning. Schools and school districts may provide funding in order to make these focusing tools more accessible to students and families. Children who use these stimuli may see an increase in long-term focusing levels, and therefore an increase in learning abilities. This study may encourage the use of stimuli in online classrooms, rather than deterring their use in “typical” children without specialized learning plans.
The initial hypothesis is accepted, as added stimuli did in fact increase focusing levels; however, the initial hypothesis failed to account for how a history with a stimuli would impact its effectiveness.
Future investigations should study the impact of these stimuli on children over a longer study period. The stimuli should be studied in the online learning environment and the in-person learning environment to determine if the same stimuli are effective per student regardless of their location or environment.