Sure. Here are some more details about the critical age hypothesis:
The critical period is believed to be between the ages of 2 and 12. This is because the brain undergoes a period of rapid development during this time, and it is thought that this development is necessary for language learning.
Children who are exposed to a second language from a young age are more likely to achieve native-like fluency than adults who learn the same language later in life. This is thought to be because children's brains are more plastic and able to adapt to new linguistic input.
There is some evidence to support the critical age hypothesis. For example, studies have shown that children who are exposed to a second language from a young age have better pronunciation than adults who learn the same language later in life. They also tend to make fewer grammatical errors.
However, the critical age hypothesis is not without its critics. Some argue that the evidence is not strong enough to support the claim that there is a critical period for language learning. They also point out that there are many adults who achieve native-like fluency in second languages.
The critical age hypothesis may not be the same for all languages. Some languages may be easier to learn than others, regardless of age.
The critical period may be affected by other factors, such as motivation and immersion. Children who are motivated to learn a second language and who are immersed in a language-rich environment may be able to achieve native-like fluency even if they are past the critical period.
Overall, the critical age hypothesis is a controversial hypothesis. There is some evidence to support it, but more research is needed to determine whether there is a true critical period for language learning.
Here are some additional thoughts on the topic of the critical age hypothesis:
The critical period may not be a single, sharp cutoff point. It is possible that there is a gradual decline in the ability to learn a language fluently as people get older.
The critical period may not apply to all aspects of language learning. For example, adults may be able to learn new vocabulary and grammar rules just as easily as children, but they may have more difficulty acquiring native-like pronunciation.
The critical period may be affected by other factors, such as the amount of exposure to the language and the individual's motivation. Children who are exposed to a second language from a young age and who are motivated to learn the language are more likely to achieve native-like fluency, even if they are past the critical period.
It is important to note that the critical age hypothesis is just a hypothesis. It is not a proven fact. There is still much that we do not know about language learning and the factors that affect it. More research is needed to determine whether there is a true critical period for language learning and how it affects different people.