First Language Acquisition: The process through which infants and young children learn their native language, typically occurring rapidly and without formal instruction.
Innate Predisposition: The inherent ability or capacity in human infants to acquire language, suggesting that language learning is a natural instinct.
Self-expression and Communication: The ability to convey thoughts, desires, and emotions through language, which develops significantly in children before formal education begins.
Language Acquisition: The process through which individuals, particularly children, learn to speak and understand a language. This involves exposure to language and interaction with other language users.
Cultural Transmission: The process of learning language through the cultural and language-using environment rather than through genetic inheritance.
Interaction: Communication with other language users, which is essential for learning a language.
Phonetic Capability: The physical ability of a child to produce sound signals necessary for speech, which is contingent on their hearing ability.
American Sign Language (ASL): A visual language used by the deaf community, which involves hand signs, facial expressions, and gestures.
INPUT
Caregiver Speech (Motherese/Child-directed speech): A simplified and exaggerated style of speech used by adults when interacting with infants and young children.
Input: The language samples provided by older children and adults, which play a crucial role in the infant's language development.
CAREGIVER SPEECH
Conversational Structure: The way in which dialogue is organized between participants, even in the presence of non-verbal cues from young children.
Interactive Role: The engagement and participation of children in conversations, highlighting their contributions through actions and vocalizations.
Features of caregiver speech
-frequent questions
-exaggerated intonation
-extra loudness
-shower tempo
-longer pauses
-treating actions and vocalizations as conversational turns
-baby talk
-simple sentence structures
-a lot of repetition
Four typical features of caregiver speech :
-Simplified sentences: Short, simple sentences like "Look!" make language easier for the child to understand.
-Repetition and paraphrasing: Caregivers frequently repeat and rephrase words to reinforce meaning.
-Exaggerated intonation: Caregivers use a lively tone to highlight important words and engage the child.
-Interactive turn-taking: Caregivers treat the child’s sounds and gestures as conversational turns, encouraging interaction.
E.g.,
M O T H E R : Look!
C H I L D : (touches pictures)
M O T H E R : What are those?
C H I L D : (vocalizes a babble string and smiles)
M O T H E R : Yes, there are rabbits.
C H I L D : (vocalizes, smiles, looks up at mother)
M O T H E R : (laughs) Yes, rabbit.
C H I L D : (vocalizes, smiles)
M O T H E R : Yes. (laughs)
Language Development: The process through which children acquire the ability to understand and use language, typically occurring in a predictable timeline.
Biologically Determined Development: Growth patterns and skill acquisition governed by biological and neurological maturation processes.
Linguistic Input: The verbal stimuli that children are exposed to, which they analyze and process to develop language skills.
Sucking Behavior: A natural reflex in infants used to measure their attention and response to auditory stimuli, indicating their interest in sound patterns.
COOING (0-5 months)
Cooing: The earliest form of speech-like sounds produced by infants, characterized by vowel-like sequences.
Vowel-like Sounds: Sounds that resemble vowels, particularly those that are high in pitch, such as [i] and [u].
Velar Consonants: Consonant sounds produced with the back of the tongue against the soft palate, represented by [k] and [ɡ].
Speech Perception: The ability to hear and differentiate between various speech sounds, including vowels and syllable formations.
For example: A 4-month-old baby makes “ooo” and “aaa” sounds, especially when seeing their parent’s face. They smile and coo in response to their parent’s expressions.
BABBLING (6-10 months)
Babbling: The production of various vowel and consonant sounds by infants, typically beginning around six to eight months. This includes the repetition of sounds (e.g., "ba-ba-ba").
Intonation Patterns: The variation in pitch and tone that can be discerned in vocalizations, developing around nine to ten months, helping make speech sound more communicative.
Pre-Language: The stage in which a child uses vocalizations to express emotions or emphasis before the formation of structured language.
Linguistic Variation: The acknowledgment that individual children may develop language skills at different ages, emphasizing the variability in language development.
For example: An 8-month-old sits up and starts babbling sounds like “ba-ba-ba” and “da-da-da.” They play with their sounds, often giggling as they vary the combinations, like “ma-ma-ma.”
THE ONE-WORD STAGE (12-18 months)
One-word stage: A developmental phase in children (between 12-18 months) characterized by the production of single recognizable utterances.
Holophrastic speech: A single word that conveys a complete idea or function as a phrase/sentence, used to express various meanings or concepts.
Single-unit utterance: An expression consisting of a single term, often used to refer to specific objects or entities but can represent broader meanings.
For example: A 15-month-old points at their bottle and says, “milk.” They also say “dog” when they see the family pet, using single words to label things around them.
THE TWO-WORD STAGE (18-24 months)
Two-word Stage: A developmental phase in early childhood where children begin combining two distinct words to form utterances.
Vocabulary: The range of words a child understands and uses; increases significantly by age two.
Functional Consequences: The real-world effects and interactions that result from the child’s utterances, which help confirm their communicative value.
For example: A 20-month-old sees their favorite toy and says, “big truck,” or holds their cup up and says, “more juice.” The parent interprets “big truck” as admiration and “more juice” as a request, responding appropriately.
TELEGRAPHIC SPEECH (2-2.5 months)
Multiple-word speech: Refers to the stage where children start producing utterances consisting of multiple words, characterized by increased variation in word forms rather than just word count.
Telegraphic speech: A developmental stage in young children’s speech characterized by short phrases or sentences that contain only essential words (lexical morphemes), omitting smaller grammatical elements.
Inflections: Variations in word forms that indicate grammatical features, such as -ing for ongoing actions.
Vocabulary growth: The rapid increase in the number of words a child can understand and use as they develop.
For example: A 2.5-year-old says, “mommy come here” when wanting attention or “me want cookie” to request a snack. They start using simple grammar forms and can combine multiple words in the correct order, although still omitting smaller words like “a” or “is.”
Linguistic repertoire: The set of language skills and vocabulary that a child possesses.
Instruction: Formal teaching or guidance on how to use language.
Imitation: The process through which children replicate words or phrases they hear from adults.
Active construction: The idea that children learn language by creating their own understanding and usage rather than passively receiving information.
Example : A child named Noah invents the verb "to Woodstock," demonstrating their ability to generate new language based on context.
LEARNING THROUGH IMITATION?
Speech Production: The process by which individuals produce spoken language. In this context, it refers to how young children articulate their thoughts and understandings.
Sentence Structures: The grammatical arrangements of words in sentences. The study indicates that children do not replicate these structures accurately, even when they can repeat words or phrases.
Examples:
"The dogs are hungry" → "dog hungry."
"The owl who eats candy runs fast" → "owl eat a candy and he run fast."
LEARNING THROUGH CORRECTION?
Corrections: Refers to attempts by adults to correct children's speech, particularly in grammar and word usage.
Sound and Word Combinations: The ways in which children create and experiment with sounds and phrases, contributing to their language development.
Telegraphic Stage: A phase in child language development characterized by the use of short, simple phrases that typically convey essential meaning, often leaving out less critical words.
Overgeneralization: Child applies a grammatical rule too broadly, leading to incorrect forms (e.g., “foots” instead of “feet”).
Sequence of Morphological Development:
-ing (e.g., "cat sitting")
Prepositions in/on (interchangeable order)
Plural -s (e.g., "cats"), with overgeneralization like "foots"
Irregular past tense (e.g., "went"), sometimes overgeneralized ("wented")
Possessive -’s (e.g., "Karen’s bed")
Verb "to be" (is/are)
Articles a/the
Past tense -ed (e.g., "walked")
Present tense -s (e.g., "comes")
Stages of Development: Refers to the identifiable phases children go through in acquiring language skills, particularly in forming questions and using negatives.
Questions and Negatives: Elements of speech where questions inquire information and negatives express denial or the absence of something.
FORMING QUESTIONS
Wh-forms: Question words such as "Who," "What," "Where," "When," "Why," and "How" used to form questions.
Rising Intonation: A speech pattern where the pitch of the voice rises at the end of a statement or question, often indicating a question in children’s speech.
Inversion: The change in the position of the auxiliary verb and the subject in a question, e.g., changing "I can have" to "Can I have."
Stage 1 (18-26 months)
Method: Simple addition of wh-forms or rising intonation.
Examples:
“Where mommy?”
“Go park?”
Stage 2 (22-30 months)
Method: More complex expressions with continued rising intonation; introduction of more wh-forms (What, Why).
Examples:
“You want play?”
“What that noise?”
Stage 3 (24-40 months)
Method: Use of inversion for auxiliary verbs; still some non-adult forms present.
Examples:
“Can I go outside?”
“Why is the sky blue?” (non-adult form: “Why dog can’t jump?”)
FORMING NEGATIVES
Negatives: Refers to words that negate the meaning of a sentence, commonly including "no," "not," "don’t," and "can’t."
Stage 1: Initial phase of negative usage where children place "No" or "Not" at the beginning of utterances.
Stage 2: The phase where children use “don’t” and “can’t” with increasingly more complex sentence structures, often attaching negatives before verbs.
Stage 3: Advanced phase where children incorporate other auxiliary verbs (e.g., “didn’t,” “won’t”) in their negative constructions, often moving away from Stage 1 structures.
Non-adult forms: These are constructions that children create that may not conform to standard adult grammatical forms.
Stage 1 (18-26 months)
Method: Use of "No" or "Not" at the beginning of the sentence; can attach to nouns and verbs.
Examples:
“No juice”
“Not a dog”
Stage 2 (22-30 months)
Method: Placement of negatives (no, not) in front of verbs; introduction of "don’t" and "can’t."
Examples:
“He no hit me”
“That not funny
Stage 3 (24-40 months)
Method: Incorporation of additional auxiliary forms (didn’t, won’t) while earlier forms may persist.
Examples:
“She didn’t saw it”
“I won’t eat that” (non-adult form: “This not mine”)
Holophrastic Stage: The early stage of language development in children where they use single words to express whole ideas or meanings.
Overextension: A phenomenon where children apply a word too broadly, using it to refer to a variety of unrelated objects based on perceived similarities.
Example: A child might call everything shiny "bow-wow" because it has something in common with a dog (maybe shiny eyes or fur).
LATER DEVELOPMENTS
Antonymous Relations: The relationship between words that have opposite meanings (e.g., "more" vs. "less").
Complex Structures: Advanced grammatical forms and extended discourse, typically developed later in childhood.
1 Can you describe four typical features of caregiver speech?
- **Simplified vocabulary and grammar**: Caregivers often use short, simple sentences with basic vocabulary.
- **Higher pitch and exaggerated intonation**: Caregivers use a higher pitch and more expressive intonation to capture the infant’s attention.
- **Repetition**: Words and phrases are frequently repeated to reinforce understanding.
- **Clear articulation and slower speech**: Speech is slower, with clear pronunciation, to make it easier for infants to process.
2 At what age is an infant capable of distinguishing between [ba] and [pa]?
- Infants can typically distinguish between sounds like [ba] and [pa] by around **1 month of age**.
3 Why are some of the infant’s first sounds described as “cooing”?
- Infants’ early sounds are called "cooing" because they involve vowel-like sounds that are soft and repetitive, often produced in a relaxed state and reflecting comfort or contentment.
4 During which period do children produce holophrastic speech?
- Holophrastic speech, where a single word conveys an entire phrase or sentence, typically occurs around **12 to 18 months** of age.
5 During which stage do children typically first produce syllable sequences similar to mama and dada and how old are they?
- This typically happens during the **babbling stage**, around **6 to 9 months** of age, when infants produce repeated syllable sequences.
6 At about what age do children typically begin producing varied syllable combinations such as ma-da-ga-ba?
- Around **8 to 10 months**, infants start producing varied syllable combinations in what’s often called “variegated babbling.”
7 Which of these expressions is likely to be used before the others?
mommy books or mommy’s book or mommy reading or mommy goed
- **"Mommy reading"** is likely to be used before the others, as it represents a simple verb-object combination that appears earlier than possessive constructions or complex phrases.
8 Which of these expressions is likely to be used before the others?
What book name? or How that opened? or Where kitty go?
- **"Where kitty go?"** is likely to come first, as children tend to ask questions about location and movement early on.
9 Which of these two utterances was produced by the older child and why?
(a) I not hurt him
(b) No the sun shining
- **(a) "I not hurt him"** is likely produced by the older child because it demonstrates an emerging understanding of sentence structure and the use of "I," which indicates a more advanced development stage than the simpler negation in (b).
10 What is the term used to describe the process involved when a child uses one word like ball to refer to an apple, an egg, a grape and a ball?
- The process of using a word like "ball" to refer to various round objects is known as **overextension**.