COPY

PRAISE FOR NUTRITION IN CRISIS 



“Fascinating book, full of original, interesting material by one of the original low-carb 

researchers whose grounding in the field goes back decades. Feinman combines a 

unique perspective with the experienced eye of a scientist. He’s had a front row seat 

to developments in the field of carbohydrates and metabolism over the past decades, 

which has given him key insights into the latest research.” 


—Nina Teicholz, author of The Big Fat Surprise 



“With his usual cutting wit and verve, Dr. Feinman hacks through the tangled thicket 

of nutritional science to show us how we got to the sorry state we find ourselves in 

today, deploying his vast experience in biochemistry, nutrition, and the turgid mind¬ 

set of academia to demystify mainstream nutritional research and explain how it has 

currently gone ofF the rails. With a sprinkling of humor and literary allusions, along 

with a deep dive into some of the nutritional literature, he tells readers what they 

need to know to shed excess body fat, lower blood sugar, and restore their health. A 

must-read for anyone with a serious interest in health and nutrition.” 


—Michael R. Eades, MD, author of Protein Power 



“We have arrived at the most important nutritional crossroads in all of human history: 

We must choose to either keep the dietary status quo despite every bit of evidence 

that it has failed, or demand a change in order to salvage what is left of our collective 

health. In Nutrition in Crisis, Dr. Richard Feinman has dropped the opening salvo 

in what is sure to be a battle for the ages—one that determines the future of how we 

eat and the prevalence of disease in the years to come. Join the revolution!” 


—Jimmy Moore, health podcaster; bestselling author of Keto Clarity 


“Every scientific discipline needs a Dr. Richard David Feinman. He is just as skeptical 

about what he believes as what he disbelieves. His writing bears eloquent testimony 

to why he is such a scientific treasure.” 


—Timothy Noakes, MD, PhD, emeritus professor, 

University of Cape Town; founder, The Noakes Foundation 



As a physician who has worked with thousands of patients unraveling the mysteries 

and mythology around diet and nutrition, it is refreshing and timely to come across 

a sane voice among the misinformation perpetuated by a misaligned food industry. 



We have moved far beyond the simple ‘calorie in, calorie out 7 mentality and can now 

embrace biochemical individuality and updated research while avoiding the dogma 

of certain dietary camps.” 


—Nasha Winters, ND,FABNO, 

author of The Metabolic Approach to Cancer 



“I have devoted my medical career to normalizing blood sugars in people with diabe¬ 

tes. This is only possible with very low carbohydrate diets. Dr. Feinman explains 

why low-carb is wise even for people without diabetes. This cant-put-it-down title 

is the closest thing to a complete, popular analysis of the biochemistry of human 

nutrition that you will find and a superb lesson in how scientific studies have been 

manipulated to prove fiction . 77 


—Richard K. Bernstein, MD,FACN, FACE, CWS, 

author of Dr Bernsteins Diabetes Solution and The Diabetes Diet 



“Professor Feinman has been a leading pioneer in educating the academic community 

and lay public on the health benefits of low-carb and ketogenic diets for years. In 

addition to being a fantastic comprehensive review of practical nutritional biochem¬ 

istry, his book also tells an informative and entertaining story about what went wrong 

in the nutrition establishment and advocates for a rational solution to the problem.” 


—Dominic D'Agostino, PhD, leading scientist 

on ketogenic metabolic therapies 



“Low-carb? Low-fat? Count your calories? If you have whiplash from trying to follow 

contradictory health headlines, you’re not alone. WeVe gotten ourselves into quite a 

nutritional mess thanks to poorly done research that was even more poorly reported 

to the public. With his trademark wry humor, Dr. Feinman exposes just how shoddy 

much of the research has been and outlines how to take control of your own health.” 


—Amy Berger, MS, CNS, NTP, 

author of The Alzheimer s Antidote 



“A distinctively insightful treatise on the sad state of affairs in nutrition written by 

one of the true thought leaders. In his unique Brooklyn style, teacher and intellectual 

Dr. Richard Feinman fights against ignorance by enlightening people on the real 

science and application of nutrition.” 


—Jeff S. Volek, professor, 

Department of Human Sciences, Ohio State University 



“I’ve had the honor and privilege of knowing and working with Dr. Feinman for many 

years. IVe watched him employ his deep knowledge of biochemistry and metabolism 

to tackle the entrenched, misguided nutritional advice cemented by thirty-five years 

of the food pyramid. His book says it all.” 


—Eugene Fine, professor, Department of Radiology, 

Albert Einstein College of Medicine 



“Dr. Feinman has spent his professional life dedicated to exposing the many flaws 

found in most reporting in nutrition science. Nutrition in Crisis walks the reader 

through the fog of dubious statistical analysis and into the light of day. In doing so 

Dr. Feinman examines the science behind the low-carb debate, using humor and 

metaphor to engage the reader in the process.” 


—Miriam Kalamian,EdM,MS,CNS, 


author of Keto for Cancer 



“Making the lifestyle and nutritional changes necessary for lasting health is difficult. 

Nutrition in Crisis supplies the intellectual imperative to take control of your own 

health without resorting to symptom-suppressing medication. Read it, learn what 

you need to do, and you will be mentally empowered and physically fortified for life!” 


—Dr. Sarah Myhill, author of Sustainable Medicine and 

Diagnosis and Treatment of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome and Myalgic Encephalitis 




NUTRITION 

in CRISIS 


Flawed Studies, 

Misleading Advice, 

and the Real Science of 

Human Metabolism 


Richard David Feinman, PhD 



CHELSEA GREEN PUBLISHING 


White River Junction, Vermont 



London, UK 



DISCLAIMER: The information presented in this book is provided as an information 

resource only and is not to be used or relied on for any diagnostic or treatment purposes. 


Please consult your health care team before implementing any strategies herein. 


Copyright © 2019 by Richard David Feinman. 


All rights reserved. 


No part of this book may be transmitted or reproduced in any form by any 

means without permission in writing from the publisher. 


Originally published by NMS Press and Duck in a Boat LLC in 2014 as 

The World Turned Upside Down: The Second Low-Carbohydrate Revolution . 


This edition published by Chelsea Green Publishing, 2019. 


Acquisitions Editor: Makenna Goodman 

Developmental Editor: Nick Kaye 

Copy Editor: Deborah ITeimann 

Proofreader: Beth Kanell 

Indexer: Linda Hallinger 

Designer: Melissa Jacobson 


Printed in Canada. 


First printing February, 2019. 


10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 19 20 21 22 23 


Our Commitment to Green Publishing 


Chelsea Green sees publishing as a tool for cultural change and ecological stewardship. We strive to align our book 

manufacturing practices with our editorial mission and to reduce the impact of our business enterprise in the environ¬ 

ment. We print our books and catalogs on chlorine-free recycled paper, using vegetable-based inks whenever possible. 

This book may cost slightly more because it was printed on paper that contains recycled fiber, and we hope you'll agree 

that it’s worth it. Chelsea Green is a member of the Green Press Initiative (www.greenpressinitiative.org), a nonprofit 

coalition of publishers, manufacturers, and authors working to protect the worlds endangered forests and conserve 

natural resources. Nutrition in Crisis was printed on paper supplied by Marquis that contains 100% postconsumer 

recycled fiber. 


Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data 

Names: Feinman, Richard D., 1940- author. 


Title: Nutrition in crisis : flawed studies, misleading advice, and the real science of human metabolism / Richard 

David Feinman, PhD. 


Description: White River Junction, Vermont: Chelsea Green Publishing, 2019. 


Identifiers: LCCN 20180484311 ISBN 9781603588195 (paperback) | ISBN 9781603588201 (ebook) 


Subjects: LCSH: Nutrition—Popular works. | Diet—Popular works. | Health—Popular works. | Diet in disease—Popular 

works. | Metabolism—Popular works. | BISAC: HEALTH & FITNESS / Nutrition. | MEDICAL / Endocrinology 

& Metabolism. | HEALTH Sc FITNESS / Diets. | HEALTH Sc FITNESS / Weight Loss. | HEALTH Sc 

FITNESS / Diseases / Diabetes. | HEALTH Sc FITNESS / Diseases / Cancer. | MEDICAL / History. 

Classification: LCC RA784 .F45 2019 | DDC 613.2—dc23 

LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2018048431 


Chelsea Green Publishing 

85 North Main Street, Suite 120 

White River Junction, VT 05001 

(802)295-6300 

www.chelseagreen.com 



V3 



RECYCLED 


Paper made from 

„ recycled material 


TTSG 


www.fsc.org FSC* Cl03567 




The book is dedicated to the memory of my father, 

Max L. Feinman, MD, 


who taught me about science and about honesty 

and how much they were the same thing. 




CONTENTS 



Introduction 1 


Part 1: Setting the Stage 


1. Handling the Crisis: The Summary in Advance 19 


2. Whaddaya Know? 35 


3. The First Low-Carbohydrate Revolution 61 


Part 2: Nutrition and Metabolism 


4. Basic Nutrition: Macronutrients 73 


5. An Introduction to Metabolism 91 


6. Sugar, Fructose, and Fructophobia 105 


7. Saturated Fat: 


On Your Plate or in Your Blood? 117 


8. Hunger: What It Is, What to Do About It 123 


9. Beyond “A Calorie Is a Calorie”: 


An Introduction to Thermodynamics 131 


Part 3: The Low-Carbohydrate Diet for Disease 


10. Diabetes 151 


11. Metabolic Syndrome: The Big Pitch 167 


Part 4: The Mess in Nutritional Science 


12. The Medical Literature: 


A Guide to Flawed Studies 175 


13. Observational Studies, Association, Causality 185 


14. Red Meat and the New Puritans 197 



15. The Seventh Egg: 


When Studies Defy Common Sense 215 


16. Intention-to-Treat: 


What It Is and Why You Should Care 219 


17. The Fiend That Lies Like Truth 233 


Part 5: The Second Low-Carbohydrate 

Revolution 


18. Nutrition in Crisis 245 


19. Cancer: A New Frontier for Low-Carbohydrate 253 


20. The Future of Nutrition 261 


Acknowledgments 265 


Notes 267 


Index 275 



Introduction 



I Ve always had a weight problem. I would rarely have been considered 

fat, but I was always trying to lose weight. When I was eight years 

old, I wanted to get thinner so I could look sharp in my Brooklyn 

Dodgers uniform to impress Barbara Levy, who was the most beautiful 

girl in the world as far as I was concerned. I don't recall having any great 

success, and it was only fairly recently that I found out that Barbara Levy 

is now Barbara Boxer, former senator from California. In any case, I always 

knew that starch made me fat—oddly, I was less afraid of sugar because 

I mistakenly believed that there wasn't that much in Coca-Cola and the 

other sodas I drank. I grew up with what is usually called a poor self- 

image, and as the old joke goes, inside of every Botero is a Giacometti 

trying to get out. 


I knew from early on that it was important to cut out starch and obvi¬ 

ous solid sugar, and I made other observations about diet—for example, 

that cold cereal for breakfast made me slightly sick. Its difficult for me 

to remember exactly what I did eat in the morning. At least some of 

the time it was bacon and eggs, which, in those days, was just one of 

the things that people ate. Nobody recoiled in horror at bacon. The only 

dietary advice at the time was to eat from the different food groups, which 

were represented by a pie chart with unique symbols in each slice. The 

bottle of milk was one that stuck in my mind. I felt early on that it was not 

interesting, and I was sure that I didn't need an “expert” to tell me what 

to eat. When the USDA food pyramid was introduced, I knew it was a 

crock and I assumed that others did, too. My principles were simple: If 

you have a weight problem, bread will make you fat, and if you don't have 

a weight problem, why do you need the USDA? I thought everybody 

was in agreement on that, but obviously that wasn't the case. I'm not sure 

why people went along with all the “expert” advice. After all, everybody 

has a great deal of experience with food. We all do three experiments in 

“nutritional science” each day. 





Nutrition in Crisis 



People's compliance with dietary standards probably has to do with the 

history of medicine. Among the turning points in that history was the 

discovery of vitamins. Unlike poisons and microorganisms, vitamins were 

stuff that you had to take if you didn't want to get sick. Another inflection 

point was the identification of cigarette smoke as a causal agent in lung 

disease. In that case, even though there was a toxic agent, the associations 

were subtle and one needed statistics or other expert insights to see the 

connection. This subtlety might have given people the idea that there were 

experts who could see harm where they couldn't. 


In my youth, I simply ignored the “expert" advice. I thought that I knew 

what to eat (I was mostly right), and I saw obesity as a personal rather than 

professional question. Decades later, when I began teaching metabolism, I 

had to confront the interaction between science and nutrition. It proved to 

be more difficult than I would have guessed. 


This book is the story of my encounter with the world of nutrition, a 

story of the science of biochemistry and metabolism—how you process 

the food that you eat. It is about the application of science to daily life, 

which is what I like about the subject. If you know a little chemistry, 

you can appreciate the way that human evolution has reached into the 

mixing pot of chemical reactions to obtain energy from the environ¬ 

ment, and even if you don't know chemistry, you can see the beauty in 

the life machine. 


But there is another side to the story. In the contentious and continually 

changing stories of nutrition in the media, I encountered a discouraging 

example of the limitations of human behavior in facing truth and prevent¬ 

ing harm. The story of nutrition has proved to be an almost unbelievable 

tale of poor and irresponsible science within the medical community, one 

of the most respected parts of our society. 


However hard it is for scientists to distrust experts, it is even harder 

for the general population. I was astounded when I saw a question on 

an online diabetes site that said, “My morning oatmeal spikes my blood 

glucose. How much carbohydrate should I have?” People with diabetes 

cannot adequately metabolize dietary carbohydrate (starch and sugar) so 

it seemed like an easy question. The answer from the experts, however, was 

waffling and tedious, and it didn't include the obvious advice: “Limit your 

oatmeal consumption to a level that doesn't spike your blood glucose.” 



Introduction 





Chemistry 


When I was eight years old, my father taught me about atoms. I have one 

of those memories that might or might not be accurate: I am sitting in my 

father’s car, and he is telling me that the whole world is made of atoms 

in the same way that the apartment building across the street is made of 

bricks. Whether or not the scene really took place, it was a major influence, 

and chemistry has long been a defining feature of my life. (Other vivid 

memories of my early life in Brooklyn—being at Ebbetts Field and seeing 

Jackie Robinson hit an inside-the-ballpark home run—turned out not to 

be true. He had hit only one, in 1948, before I had ever seen a live game). 


The crux of atomic theory, the thing that captures everybody’s imagina¬ 

tion when they are first exposed to it, is that it is a global and absolute 

theory—it explains everything that has been done in the laboratory, the 

kitchen, or anywhere else. Various fields of chemistry get at that same sense 

of universal understanding with differing degrees of intellectual rigor, but 

eventually I recognized that biochemistry was a good place to be for a 

young person who didn’t know what career he wanted to end up with. 

You can do drug design or theoretical chemistry or animal behavior or 

nutrition and still call yourself a biochemist. 



Teaching Nutrition 


I have worked in a number of fields in biochemistry, but it was teaching 

metabolism to medical students at SUNY Downstate Medical Center that 

led to my professional interest in nutrition. Metabolism is the study of the 

way food is processed and of the biochemical reactions that control life 

functions. It is a fairly complicated subject—those parts that we understand 

at all. Because there are so many individual biochemical reactions, students 

tend to see the subject in the same way that somebody once described 

the study of history: just one damn thing after another. There are general 

principles and big concepts, of course, but you do have to know the details. 

When I began teaching metabolism, I used the low-carbohydrate 

diet—at the time primarily a weight-loss diet—as a central element in 

my teaching. Control of blood glucose and insulin, the hormone whose 

release is controlled by glucose, is central to many different processes in 





Nutrition in Crisis 



biochemistry. In the complicated network of biochemical reactions, insulin 

stands out as a major point of regulation. The ups and downs of insulin are 

what we try to control through the use of dietary carbohydrate restriction 

as a therapeutic method. So, low-carbohydrate diets provided some unify¬ 

ing theme in teaching. I still teach metabolism in this way, though I now 

emphasize diabetes where impaired ability to handle dietary carbohydrate 

is the salient feature. The low-carbohydrate diet and its more thorough 

form, the ketogenic diet, are popular—periodically very popular—and 

while they remain controversial, the number of adherents, and possibly the 

desperation of the detractors, suggests that low-carbohydrate must inevi¬ 

tably be accepted as I will describe it: the “default” diet for diabetes (the 

one to try first) and the best diet for weight loss for many people. It is likely 

that its current popularity can't be turned back. Myself and others who use 

this teaching method have published papers about how understanding the 

real-world benefits of low-carbohydrate—getting control of your health 

and regulating your weight—can help you learn chemistry. 1 


Around 2000, one of our second-career medical students who had been 

a dietitian suggested that we include formal nutrition in our biochemistry 

course, and she provided subject matter from standard nutritional practice. 

I cannot really describe what it was about—probably, even at the time, 

it was so vacuous that I couldn't retain it in my memory. In any case, I 

objected because whatever it was, it wasn't biochemistry. The way I saw it, 

criticizing how lectures are given is like complaining about how the dishes 

are done: Everybody sees an immediate solution. Despite my protests, I 

wound up having to give formal lectures in nutrition, and I really didn't 

know the literature. I had long ago found that carbohydrate restriction was 

best for me, and while low-carbohydrate diets provided me with a good 

framework for teaching metabolism, applied therapies do not always have 

a close relation to theories, so teaching nutrition required a certain amount 

of background study on my part. 


My first lectures on nutrition were neutral. I simply tried to cover 

the basic aspects of low-carbohydrate and low-fat diets—the two main 

choices, really—presenting the pros and cons of each approach in a simple 

way. Low-fat diets are not based substantially on biochemical mechanisms; 

instead, they follow from observed correlations between cardiovascu¬ 

lar disease and the presence of cholesterol or other lipids in the blood. 



Introduction 





More recently, low-fat has morphed into a prescription for obesity, and 

proponents have started emphasizing the point that fat has more calories 

per gram than other things, peddling the idea that the more calories, the 

greater the effect on body weight—the ill-conceived idea that “you are 

what you eat,” which hangs over everything. While I could explain at that 

time how metabolism, and specifically the role of the hormone insulin, 

accounted for the benefits of a low-carb diet, I could not provide a well- 

organized review of the relevant studies in the medical literature. So, my 

initial lectures were rather simple and straightforward while I tried to get 

a grip on the scientific literature. 


As I dug into that literature, however, it didn’t take long to see that 

something was terribly wrong. In simply trying to grasp the facts, I had 

stepped into a world of bad science, self-deception, and a scandal equal to 

any in the history of medicine. 



The Nurses’ Health Study 


Science is very specialized. Although I had been doing research on blood 

coagulation, which is related to cardiovascular disease (CVD), I did not 

pay much attention to the diet-heart hypothesis—the idea that fat and 

cholesterol in the diet raise blood cholesterol, which, in turn, leads to CVD. 

I was suspicious of such a theory, though, because biology tends to run on 

hormones and enzymes—that is to say, on control mechanisms rather than 

on mass action (the principle that chemical processes are determined by 

how much reactants are put into them). The grand principle in biochem¬ 

istry is that there is hardly anything that is not connected with feedback. 

If you try to lower your dietary cholesterol, for example, your liver will 

respond by making more. Simply adding more or less is not guaranteed 

to produce much change at all, once feedback is taken into account. I was 

therefore skeptical, if not well-informed. 


Whatever my misgivings about the diet-heart hypothesis, I didn’t ques¬ 

tion it very deeply at first. However, when I went back to the original 

literature to find the evidence supporting low-fat recommendations, 

as I had to do in preparation for my lectures, it was a rude awakening. 

My assumption that there was at least a grain of truth in the diet-heart 

hypothesis turned out to be overly optimistic. If the hypothesis is not a 





Nutrition in Crisis 



Replace Lower Risk Higher Risk 



Saturated fat ._ 


-4 fco/_ nf onprnvl 


with carbohydrates 


l /o ui cl ici y yj 


Monounsaturated fat 

with carbohydrates 





Polyunsaturated fat 

with carbohydrates 


H- --- ■ - //— 



Saturated fat with 



monounsaturated fat 




Saturated fat with 




polyunsaturated fat 



-1-1 1 



1—-1-1-1- I 1 ' 1 


-SO -60 -40 -20 0 20 40 60 80 


Change in Risk [%) 



Figure 0.1. Estimated changes in risk of coronary heart disease associated with isoca¬ 

loric substitutions [error bars show 95 percent confidence interval]. Adapted from F. 

B. Hu et al„ “Dietary Fat Intake and the Risk of Coronary Heart Disease in Women," New 

England Journal of Medicine 337, no. 21 [1997]: 1491-1499. 


total sham, it is pretty close. One of the first papers that I came across in 

my literature survey was a report from the Nurses' Health Study (NHS). 

Centered at Harvard, the NHS is one of the largest epidemiological stud¬ 

ies with more than one hundred thousand participants. It has produced a 

large number of studies on nutrition and other aspects of lifestyle. 


Walter Willett, head of the NHS and follow-up studies, and his associ¬ 

ate Frank Hu examined the association of different kinds of fat, as well 

as carbohydrate, with the risk of CVD. 2 I found the result astounding. 

Figure 0.1, redrawn from their paper, shows the effects of substituting one 

type of fat for another, and of substituting carbohydrate for fat. Replacing 

saturated fat with either polyunsaturated fat (vegetable oils) or monoun- 

saturated fat (olive or canola oil) reduced risk substantially. That's what 

the nutritional community had been saying, so I saw no surprise there. 










Introduction 





However, when the polyunsaturated fat was replaced with carbohydrate, 

Hu et al. found an average 60 percent increase in risk. What? Carbohydrate 

is worse than fat for cardiovascular risk? Thats not how it was supposed to 

be. What about saturated fat? Surely that’s a bad guy. Replacing saturated 

fat with carbohydrate did provide some benefit according to the figure, at 

least on average, but there is more to the story. In this kind of figure, the 

error bars (horizontal lines) show the spread of individual values, which 

was quite large in this case. In other words, even though there was an 

average improvement from replacing saturated fat with monounsaturated 

fat—their main selling point—some subjects experienced greater benefit 

than the average, and some much less benefit than the average. When satu¬ 

rated fat was replaced by carbohydrate, some of the study’s subjects were, in 

fact, going in the opposite direction—that is, they were at greater risk for 

CVD, which contradicted the supposed benefit of reducing fat. It wasn’t 

just a few subjects, either. The breakdown was about 60:40: 60 percent 

of subjects experienced reduced risk of CVD by replacing saturated fat 

with carbohydrate, and 40 percent experienced greater risk. It gets worse. 

Without getting too caught up in the statistical details, the rule is that if 

the (horizontal) error bar crosses the zero line, then there is no significant 

effect of the substitution. So, based on the study’s findings, substituting 

carbohydrate in place of saturated fat is at best neutral, or more precisely, it 

is as likely to increase risk as it is to lower it. The same is true of substitut¬ 

ing carbohydrate for monounsaturated fat. 


Looking at figure 0.1, it is hard to see a risk of fat, but hasn’t risk from 

fat been the message all along? Certainly the idea that carbohydrate is a 

risk is not found in the media or the pronouncements of health agencies. 

And then there is the authors’ summary of the paper: 


Our data provide evidence in support of the hypothesis that a 

higher dietary intake of saturated fat . . . is associated with an 

increased risk of coronary disease, whereas a higher intake of 

monounsaturated and polyunsaturated fats is associated with 

reduced risk. These findings reinforce evidence from metabolic 

studies that replacing saturated fat . . . with un-hydrogenated 

monounsaturated and polyunsaturated fats favorably alters the 

lipid profile, but that reducing overall fat intake has little effect. 





Nutrition in Crisis 



This conclusion is not accurate. It’s at best misleading, and at worst 

outright deceptive. The measured risk of saturated fat intake was dependent 

on what it was replaced with, yet there is no mention of carbohydrate as a 

replacement. The most striking thing to me was that if you looked at the 

risk from carbohydrate in comparison to the risk from saturated fat—that 

is, the risk of substituting one for the other—there was no difference. Even 

worse, substituting carbohydrate for other fats increased risk. How could 

this be? Fat out. Carbohydrate in. Wasn’t that the clear recommendation 

for improved health from just about every health agency and expert? Yet 

the data said it didn’t matter. Was it dishonest not to make this clear in 

the discussion section of the paper? At best, it was an error of omission. 

The authors from the Harvard School of Public Health were, and still are, 

the more modest among those vilifying fat, insisting that it is only the 

type of fat that we need worry about. Most recently, the American Heart 

Association (AHA) has come around to the same point of view as if they 

had just discovered it. I was probably not alone, but I began using the 

term lipophobes long ago for proponents of low-fat. It’s a wiseguy term, and 

since I was still something of an inside player in the nutrition world, I was 

reluctant to put it into print until Michael Pollan started using it without 

any sense of irony. 3 (I started saying “... as Michael Pollan calls them.”) 


By the time I read the NHS paper, my professional involvement in the 

field of nutrition was cemented. I did not, however, adequately attend to 

the sense of being sucked into a whirlpool from which it would be hard to 

escape. The data supporting low-carbohydrate were there for everyone to see, 

I thought, even if the authors had chosen to downplay the strongest result. 


A trip to the supermarket today demonstrates that the results from 

the Nurses’ Health Study had little effect. The low-fat story is still with 

us. More striking is that two meta-analyses (averages of many studies) 

came to the same conclusion regarding the ineffectiveness of replacing fat 

with carbohydrate. Siri-Tarino et al. concluded that “there are few epide¬ 

miologic or clinical trial data to support a benefit of replacing saturated 

fat with carbohydrate,” and in March of 2014, yet another meta-analysis 

found similar results. 4 What turned out to be most remarkable about all of 

these studies was that they presented a reanalysis of studies that had found 

no effect of saturated fat to begin with. One has to ask why the results 

were not accepted when first published. Some of the included studies are 



Introduction 





twenty years old. How is it possible that, in the most scientific period in 

history our society runs on incorrect scientific information? That's one of 

the questions that I will try to answer in this book—or at least describe, as 

Fm not sure that there is a clear-cut answer. Looking ahead, I will intro¬ 

duce the revolutionary idea that, except in cases of well-defined genetic 

abnormalities, there is no predictable effect of diet on heart disease based 

on the current research. It is a hypothesis, and we might know more as we 

understand the genetics, but no effect is certainly more plausible than the 

diet-heart hypothesis, which remains only a conjecture without experi¬ 

mental support. This lack of effect will be one of the themes in this book 

and one of the battlegrounds as the crisis in nutrition plays out. 



About This Book: Who It’s for and Why I Wrote It 


Food and chemistry have been two of the largest influences in my life. 

The beauty of biochemistry is that it relates the movement of electrons to 

whats on our plates—and this is a connection I thought I could explain. I 

like writing about biochemistry. It allows us to see how things fit together, 

but it also exposes the things we don't know—the things that evoke within 

us curiosity, the defining feature of the scientific life. If you want to indulge 

that curiosity, you are the person I had in mind when I started the book. 


This is a book for scientists. Not specifically for people with atomic- 

force microscopes in their labs, but for those who want to look at nutrition 

from a scientific point of view. Science is less about sophisticated measure¬ 

ments than it is about basic honesty. It is true that scientific fields can be 

very mathematical or intellectually rigorous, but all sciences, even those as 

complex as quantum mechanics, are tied to logic and common sense, and 

are frequently directly accessible to lay audiences. Part of the game, most 

researchers understand, is to make the results easy to understand. Einstein 

is widely quoted as having said that we want to make it simple but not 

too simple. Modern medicine, despite its reliance on technology, explicitly 

accepts an obligation to explain things logically to the patient. It doesn't 

always fulfill this obligation well, but the goal remains nonetheless. In this 

book I will try to fill some of the gaps and define words, but for the tough 

spots, you will have to read like a scientist. How do we read? We re all 

specialists so most of us cant read technical articles, even those in our own 



10 



Nutrition in Crisis 



fields, without some bumps. Skip over the bumps and see if you can get 

the big picture. You can always come back to them later, and many of the 

details are just a Google away. 


If you write a book about biochemistry, it’s about chemistry, but if you 

write a book about nutrition, it’s about everything. Not every chapter in 

this book is for everybody. I have tried to provide a continuous, easy-to- 

follow thread, but different subjects require different kinds of discussion, 

and some of these discussions are necessarily technical. You can skip them, 

but I do suggest giving them a shot. 


Although this is primarily a book about the science of nutrition, you 

can’t escape the sociology and politics of medicine. Establishment medi¬ 

cal journals, private organizations, and government health agencies have 

insisted on low-fat, low-calorie dogma despite the scientific evidence to 

the contrary. This politically motivated breakdown in scientific practice is 

deeply discouraging to me and was an additional motivation for my writing 

this book. The corruption of science goes beyond principle, too what s at 

stake is the health of patients. 


The breakthrough in understanding metabolism that underlies much of this 

book comes from the realization that many superficially unrelated patho¬ 

logic or disease states and associated conditions are intimately connected 

at the physiological and biological level. Equally important, control of 

these states rests in a major way with diet. Diabetes, obesity, cardiovascular 

disease, states of hypertension, and numerous other physiologic states are 

all tied together. The promise is that, examined together, they might provide 

a global theory of metabolism and with it a common cure. 


A major focus of this book is the concept of metabolic syndrome, which 

is a collection of clinical markers—including overweight, high blood pres¬ 

sure, and the so-called atherogenic dyslipidemia (the lipid markers that 

are assumed to contribute to cardiovascular disease)—that together and 

in combination indicate risk of disease. The identification of metabolic 

syndrome constitutes, in my view, a great intellectual insight. That the 

common effector is likely the hormone insulin points to the importance of 

controlling dietary carbohydrate, the major stimulus for insulin secretion. 


The resistance of the medical profession to dietary carbohydrate restric¬ 

tion in the treatment of metabolic syndrome, and even more obviously, 



Introduction 



11 



in the treatment of diabetes, I find incomprehensible. Everybody knows 

somebody with diabetes. Echoes of the early days in Brooklyn made it very 

upsetting to see pictures of Jackie Robinson taken shortly before his death 

from diabetes complications at age fifty-two. Because it is progressive, the 

disease is an underappreciated source of suffering. Clinicians will tell you 

that it is like cancer in its devastating effects. Diabetes is the major cause of 

amputations after accidents and the major cause of acquired blindness.That 

is a motivation for writing this book and why you might find it important. 


This resistance is a scandal at the level of Ignaz Semmelweis, an 

early-nineteenth-century Viennese physician. To reduce the incidence 

of puerperal fever (infection after childbirth), Semmelweis suggested 

that physicians wash their hands after performing autopsies and before 

delivering babies. They refused; it was too much trouble. But that was the 

nineteenth century, before the germ theory was established, and thats 

some kind of excuse. It's hard to know how we will look back on the 

actions of the American Diabetes Association (ADA), who believe that 

for people with diabetes, “Sucrose-containing foods can be substituted for 

other carbohydrates in the meal plan or, if added to the meal plan, covered 

with insulin or other glucose-lowering medications.” 5 


The most difficult part of writing this book was trying to understand—if 

such a feat is even possible-—how the whole field of medical nutrition could 

be wrong. Way wrong. Totally off the mark. As misguided as the alchemists' 

pursuit of the creation of gold. This disconnect from true science is not only 

bizarre; it is a source of real harm to patients.The phenomenon is particularly 

hard to explain because the widespread respect for the medical professionals 

is based on real accomplishment and expertise, and it is hard to see why they 

would go so wrong in nutrition. For me, having precedents makes it easier 

to grasp, if not completely comprehensible. Here's one example of self- 

deception and refusal to accept evidence that I keep in mind. The following 

is a passage from Abraham Rabinovitch's writing on the Israel Defense 

Forces and intelligence in the days before the Yom Kippur War (1973): 


The intelligence chiefs believed they knew a deeper truth ... that 

rendered irrelevant all the cries of alarm going up around them. 


Zeira and his chief aides were to demonstrate the ability of even 

brilliant men to adhere to an idee fixe in the face of mountains 



12 



Nutrition in Crisis 



of contrary evidence.... They clung to their view even though 

the Egyptian deceptions were contradicted by evidence of war 

preparations that AMAN's [military intelligence] own depart¬ 

ments were daily gathering. . . . But the deception succeeded 

beyond even Egypt's expectations because it triggered within 

Israel's intelligence arm and senior command a monumental 

capacity for self-deception. 6 


The Israelis could have lost it all. They could have lost the whole country 

due to their refusal to accept the evidence.They were largely saved, however, 

by a couple of field commanders who were wild and crazy guys most 

notably Ariel Sharon, who attacked an Egyptian emplacement by disobey¬ 

ing orders not to cross the Nile. Audacity and the refusal to follow orders 

might be what save nutrition as well. 


Finally, this book is for the person (and those for whom she spoke) who 

posted on my blog wondering how she could determine which nutritional 

studies are flawed and which are not, especially at a time when we are 

inundated with so many conflicting recommendations. “Where are the 

true studies that are NOT flawed,” she wrote, “and how do I differentiate? 


She was right to be suspicious. It is not always easy. There are so many 

nutrition papers that try to snow you with technical detail, and those are in 

fact the ones to be most suspicious of. Scientific papers will necessarily have 

technical components, but researchers shouldn't be making their results 

more difficult to understand than they need to be—and some of the papers 

are simply not true. Most researchers know that if you make up the data on 

a federally funded grant, you can go to jail, but when it comes to interpret¬ 

ing the data, they can say just about any damned thing. In this book, I 

explain how to interpret nutrition papers. In particular, I explain what the 

statistics mean, how they can be misused, and how to navigate the literature 

as someone who doesn't necessarily have a background in statistics. 



The Second Low-Carbohydrate Revolution 


The killer app, so to speak, of the low-carbohydrate diet is still the treat¬ 

ment of diabetes. Intuitively obvious, proved in many experimental 

trials, and widely used anecdotally and clinically, there are virtually no 



Introduction 



13 



contraindications. Resistance to its use appears to be spurred on entirely 

by pressure from political organizations, primarily the American Diabetes 

Association (ADA), which, looking for a way to save face, still refuses to 

endorse low-carbohydrate strategies. Many identify the influence, direct 

or indirect, of drug companies and food companies as a culprit as well. 

Whatever the motivation, not encouraging physicians to at least offer 

carbohydrate restriction is seen by many who have had success with the 

approach as “criminal.” The latest guidelines from the ADA emphasize 

“individualization,” presumably as a way of softening their previous oppo¬ 

sition to low-carbohydrate. The word individualization is used twenty-one 

times in their position paper, 7 but the actual principles to be applied for 

each individual are not stated. The foolishness of not explicitly restricting 

carbohydrate for people with a disease whose most salient manifesta¬ 

tion is an inability to adequately metabolize carbohydrate is astounding. 

Individualization, in my view, can best be described as a cop-out. 


Despite the resistance to low-carbohydrate, we have at the same time 

a constant flow of blog posts and books that show the low-fat diet-heart 

hypothesis for the intellectual and clinical disaster that it really is. The most 

recent and most complete, a book called The Big Fat Surprise* is surprising 

in its description of the depths of self-delusion, if not dishonesty, in keeping 

low-fat alive. While the pace of criticism is increasing, these exposes docu¬ 

ment that the diet-heart hypothesis has been debunked since its inception. 


If you step back and look at the data, the concerns, the voices on 

Huffington Post, or the numerous blogs belonging to dietitians, it shines 

through that the easiest way to lose weight is the low-carbohydrate diet. 

The concerns, voiced for forty years, have never been effectively substanti¬ 

ated, and the real-world tests of carbohydrate restriction come out in its 

favor. There are now dozens of successful implementations, though the 

Atkins diet is still the best known, having attained a somewhat generic 

status, like Kleenex. 



Metabolic Syndrome: The Big Pitch 


There is almost nothing in biology that is not connected with feedback. 

This idea is fundamental yet widely ignored. Reducing dietary intake of 

cholesterol will have limited effect because of compensatory synthesis. 



14 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Likewise, if you stop eating carbohydrate, your body will respond by 

synthesizing glucose and making other fuels available. This grand idea puts 

severe limitations on what you can do (as in the case of cholesterol or, look¬ 

ing ahead, trying to starve tumors by reducing glucose), but it also points 

to some opportunities. When you consume carbohydrate, the hormone 

insulin turns off the feedback system in the liver that produces glucose 

from glycogen or gluconeogenesis. Understanding that diabetes represents 

a breakdown in this feedback system—that the liver of a person with type 

2 diabetes will not respond to insulin (insulin resistance)—makes clear 

why you should not add more insulin. Nonetheless, the compensatory 

feedback response to many drugs and foods does call for caution in jump¬ 

ing to conclusions. 


The key point is that there is a global effect of the hormone insulin. 

We -can get very far simply by regulating this hormone. The role of 

feedback is part of the picture, but the effects of manipulating insulin 

can be highly predictable, which is the main theme of this book. Always 

in the background of this discussion is metabolic syndrome (MetS). 

Metabolic syndrome is rooted in the observation, generally credited to 

Gerald Reaven, an endocrinologist who died recently, that a collection 

of seemingly different physiologic effects—overweight, high blood pres¬ 

sure, high blood glucose, high insulin, and the collection of blood lipid 

markers referred to as atherogenic dyslipidemia (high triglycerides, low 

HDL)—are all tied together by a common causal thread: disruption in 

the metabolic response to insulin. 9 The physiologic markers of MetS 

predict progression to the associated disease states (obesity, diabetes, 

hypertension, and cardiovascular disease), all of which respond to dietary 

carbohydrate restriction. That is the big pitch. This observation confirms 

that it really, is a syndrome (since it has a common underlying cause) 

and simultaneously points us to the most effective treatment. No dietary 

approach is better than low-carbohydrate and no drug will target all of 

the markers together. 


There are, in fact, critics of MetS who question the practical significance 

of the syndrome. What they’re really suggesting is that the effects have to 

be treated with a collection of drugs: drugs for diabetes, drugs for heart 

disease, drugs for high blood pressure. A low-carbohydrate diet, which is 

already widely accepted as effective for weight loss, is likely the strategy 



Introduction 



15 



to treat the different facets of MetS without using this cocktail of drugs. 

Acceptance of such a notion is the goal of the revolution. 


Oddly enough, the bright light on the horizon is the ketogenic diet for 

cancer. I say “oddly” because carbohydrate restriction for diabetes is already 

a slam-dunk, and should have been the crystallizing point for change. Of 

course, as I’ve already discussed, the resistance to low-carbohydrate for 

people with diabetes has been extensive. Somehow treatment of cancer 

is not encountering the same obstinacy, despite limited research on the 

subject. In chapter 19 I describe work by my colleague Dr. Eugene J. Fine, 

targeting insulin in the treatment of cancer. I see this study, despite its 

small size, as a reason for hope and a sign of future progress. 10 If it turns 

out that we learn to treat diabetes by learning to treat cancer, it would not 

be the strangest thing that ever happened in science. 


How to Approach This Book 


If we had ham, we could have ham and eggs , if we had eggs, 


—Old American idiom 


This book represents the view of a practicing biochemist, and as such, it 

approaches nutrition as applied biochemistry. Biochemistry is not all there 

is to nutrition, but it represents a more scientific and logical perspective 

than “you are what you eat”; it tells us instead that we are what our metab¬ 

olism does with what we eat. I will explain why low-carbohydrate diets are 

the default diet (the one to try first) for diabetes and metabolic syndrome 

and why you need to understand this idea even if you are not suffering 

from either condition. To be clear, I am not an advocate of anything. In the 

end, you have to be your own nutritionist. My job is to give you some tools 

for sorting out the army of nutritional “experts” out there and the studies 

they produce. 


I am not an expert on politics, but, as in the aphorism most often attrib¬ 

uted to.John Adams (I think he stole it from the ancients), I study politics so 

that my children can study biochemistry and nutrition. It’s all tied together. 

The science is not divorced from the politics. The Framingham Study, 11 

the first very large population trial, tested not only a scientific principle— 

whether dietary fat and cholesterol were related to risk of cardiovascular 



16 



Nutrition in Crisis 



disease (they were not)—but also whether the recommendations of health 

agencies were a rush to judgment (they were). The study had such a large 

political component that, as striking as the scientific outcome was—there 

was no effect of dietary total or saturated fat or cholesterol on cardiovas¬ 

cular disease—it couldn’t be seen to fail. The results were buried for years 

until a statistician rediscovered them and finally had them published. This 

intertwining of the politics and science is a persistent pattern, and I try 

to tell both sides of the story and explain how they connect to each other. 


My main principle, however, is that basic science comes first. I give 

preference to the demonstrations in nutritional and medical practice that 

are based in the fundamentals of biochemistry, of hard science. Big clinical 

trials have to be judged on their inherent strength, but, if they contradict 

basic science, the authors have an obligation to explain why. And science is 

continuous with common sense. It doesn’t matter how many statistical tests 

you run: If the results violate common sense, it is unlikely to be science. 


The poor research published by prestigious individuals and institutions 

suggests the nature of science itself has to be investigated. To do this, we 

will have to define scientific principles, explain how to read a scientific 

paper, and determine whether peer review has done its job. But first, in 

chapter 1,1 give you the bottom line—the practical consequences of the 

science. The rest of the book will serve to justify these statements and 

recommendations. 



PART 1 


Setting the Stage 




-CHAPTER 1- 



Handling the Crisis 


The Summary in Advance 



W hat should I eat? This question invariably comes up during 

my lectures to medical students, during presentations at 

conferences, and even in private conversations about scientific 

experiments. Depending on my audience, I will go into different levels of 

technical detail, but the bottom line is always the same: The best diet is the 

one that works. Any expert can tell you how his or her diet theoretically 

conforms to widely accepted science, and how it is “healthy,” “moderate,” 

and a “bargain”—but if it doesn’t work in practice, if you don’t lose weight, 

if your blood sugar doesn’t go down, then it’s no good at all. 


There is little evidence that the diets recommended by government 

and private health agencies have provided much help for the current 

epidemic of obesity and diabetes, but they keep pushing them anyway. 

Defenders usually tell you that it is because people are not really 

following the guidelines. What they don’t say is how they know the 

recommendations are good if nobody follows them. So here I’ll give you 

three basic rules of nutrition that I propose as a guide, and I’ll show you a 

few principles that will help you follow them. My recommendations are 

likely different from what your doctor has told you. The rest of the book 

will justify these principles. 



The Three Rules 


The following are three simple, fundamental principles for getting control 

of your diet: 



Rule 1. If you’re OK, you’re OK. 



20 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Rule 2. If you want to lose weight, don’t eat. If you have to eat, 

don’t eat carbs. 


Rule 3. If you have diabetes or metabolic syndrome, carbohydrate 

restriction is the “default” approach—that is, the one to try first. 


RULE 1. If you don’t have a weight problem, if you feel OK, if you 

are healthy, and if you don’t have a family history of disease, there is 

no compelling reason to change your diet. Rule 1 is actually surprising 

to many people. You might want to find out more about nutrition and 

biochemistry, but the idea that there was once a Garden of Eden diet that 

we all ate until somebody first brought high-fructose corn syrup into the 

world, along with all our woe, seems unlikely. 


Not everybody has this view. There is the idea, not always stated explic¬ 

itly, that, analogous to Freud’s The Psychopathology of Everyday Life , we are 

all doing something wrong and that life is a continuous battle between 

what our bodies really need and the pressures of civilization. It’s not like 

that. We evolved to be adaptable. Lots of dietary approaches work and, 

when it comes down to it, none of us are going to get out of this alive. 


And then there are the Dietary Guidelines for Americans from the 

USDA. Congress specifically charges the department with providing 

advice to people who are healthy—that is, people who don’t need advice. In 

Brooklyn, we call this fixing something that ain’t broke. Like psychoana¬ 

lysts, these government nutritionists feel endowed by their Creator with 

the intuitive power to penetrate unspoken, unmeasured, deep truths, and 

believe that they are able to tell us that we are not eating the right thing and 

that we are at risk for some future disease. They are, however, quick to take 

offense if you suggest that their inability to control the epidemic of obesity 

and diabetes makes it very unlikely that they know what the right thing is. 


RULE 2. If you want to lose weight, don’t eat. If you have to eat, don’t 

eat carbs. I first said this as a joke at a conference, but there is a great deal of 

truth in it. This is not to say that starvation is a good long-term strategy— 

the danger is that you will lose muscle mass along with your fat but too 

frequently we think that it is important to eat all the time. That is not true, 

and intermittent fasting, which is garnering a certain amount of inter¬ 

est, might be a very useful strategy for weight loss. There are exceptions: 

Some medical conditions, diabetes in particular, might require individual 



Handling the Crisis 



21 



variation. (Calorie reduction is beneficial for diabetes, but the need to avoid 

ups and downs means that there are other considerations.) The problem, 

though, is that we tend to think that hunger is some kind of physiologic 

signal telling us that our body needs food. We think that this signal must 

be answered immediately. Its not like that. Chapter 8 explains how hunger 

only means that you are in a situation where you are used to eating. This 

situation, such as a business lunch or a tailgate party, might have little to 

do with your state of nourishment. The hunger pangs that you feel might 

be real enough, but you are not compelled to answer them—and they don't 

even last that long. 


Calories are a measure of the total energy available from burning food. 

The less food you eat, the less energy you will have. Not all calories are the 

same, though. Many experiments that show success with calorie reduction, 

on closer analysis, reveal that the effect was due to the de facto reduc¬ 

tion in carbohydrate. Some dietary strategies will waste more energy than 

others (as heat and other unproductive effects).These nuances make up the 

second part of Rule 2, but the fundamental truth remains that if you don't 

eat, you will get thin. 


RULE 3. If you have diabetes or metabolic syndrome, carbohydrate 

restriction is the “default” approach—that is, the one to try first. Almost 

everyone is now within two degrees of separation of somebody who has 

diabetes. Diabetes is a disease of carbohydrate intolerance. In type 1, there 

is a substantially reduced or total inability of the pancreas to produce 

insulin in response to carbohydrate. Type 2 is characterized by insulin 

resistance—the inability of the body's cells to properly respond to the 

insulin that is produced—as well as progressive deterioration of the insu¬ 

lin-producing cells in the pancreas.The defining symptom and major cause 

of the pathology is high blood sugar. The idea for treatment is simple: If 

diabetes is a disease of carbohydrate intolerance, it is reasonable to assume 

that adding dietary carbohydrate would make things worse, and restricting 

dietary carbohydrate would make things better. It makes sense, and this 

expectation is generally borne out. There are people with diabetes who can 

tolerate greater or lesser amounts of carbohydrate but, for most people, 

this simple treatment works exactly as it's supposed to—and dietary carbo¬ 

hydrate restriction might even be best for those people who can tolerate 

higher carbohydrate. There are no experimental or clinical data that show 



22 



Nutrition in Crisis 



a contradiction. The fact that people with diabetes are not typically offered 

a low-carbohydrate diet as the default treatment, let alone as an option at 

all, is in my view a major scandal in the history of medicine. 


Keep in mind that if you are on diabetes medication, you have to discuss 

getting on a low-carbohydrate diet with your physician. Carbohydrate 

restriction will lower blood glucose in the same way as many medications, 

so a low-carbohydrate diet in conjunction with your current medication 

regime might pose a risk of hypoglycemia (low blood sugar). Generally 

your physician, if he or she has any experience with carbohydrate restric¬ 

tion, will reduce or eliminate your medication before putting you on a 

low-carbohydrate diet. Some people see this as the single best argument 

for carbohydrate restriction: In most diseases, reduction in medication is 

considered a sign of success. 



Doing It: Eat to the Meter 


“Eat to the meter” is the principle used by people with diabetes. The 

meter is the glucometer, which, as you can probably guess, measures blood 

glucose. If the food you just ate causes a spike in blood sugar, it is a sign to 

avoid that food. Simple as that. Oddly, diabetes educators might tell you 

that if a food spikes your blood glucose, it means you need more insulin to 

deal with that food—a truly misguided notion. 


Those of us with a weight problem might sensibly eat to what I call 

the ponderometer: the bathroom scale. If the experts tell you to eat more 

whole grains, but the reading on the scale keeps going up, then stop! 


Doing It: 


The Best Exercise Is the One That You Do 


The nutrition world can’t agree on much, but there are few who would 

dispute that exercise is inherently beneficial. Exercise by itself is not 

particularly effective for weight loss unless you’re a professional athlete or 

in basic training, but it does enhance the benefits of dieting. Some of the 

effects are vascular—that is, physiologic rather than biochemical so it is 

hard to pin down the relation to nutrition, but finding any agreement in 

this contentious field is a good thing. 



Handling the Crisis 



23 



A Brief Primer on Carbohydrate Restriction 


Calories count, but the advice by experts that only calories count 

is wrong. There are great advantages to diet strategies that go 

beyond calories. For most people the best strategy for weight 

loss is to reduce carbohydrate intake. Insistence on the idea 

that “a calorie is a calorie,” as it is usually stated, is one of the 

crystallizing points of the nutritional crisis. The benefits of a low- 

carbohydrate, higher-fat diet are due partly to the more satiating 

effects of protein and fat, or, more precisely, the poor satiation 

from most carbohydrate. Its important to keep in mind that 

“a calorie is a calorie” is not true as a general principle; instead, 

it is the macronutrient composition of the diet that affects the 

amount of weight gained or lost. There are extensive data in the 

medical literature to support the unique effects of carbohydrate 

reduction, but the best evidence might be anecdotal. In the 

field of weight loss, anecdotal evidence is actually fairly reliable. 

Everybody knows somebody who lost a lot of weight on the 

Atkins diet. Some report that the pounds seem to u melt away” 

There is no guarantee that you will have the same experience, 

and everybody hits a plateau, but it is still your best bet. Those 

who give dire warnings about low-carb diets likely haven’t actu¬ 

ally recommended them to patients or tried them personally. 


Low-carbohydrate and ketogenic diets follow* basic biochem¬ 

istry. The key factor is improved control of insulin, the anabolic 

(building up) hormone that controls the major events in 

metabolism—storing fat and carbohydrate as glycogen, encour- 

aging protein synthesis—and insulin is most reliably controlled 

by carbohydrate. Diets based on reduced carbohydrate are 

consistently successful and that's why people keep using them 

in the face of ^concerns” of the medical establishment. As in 

any diet, people might quit at a certain point—we have six 

hundred million years of evolution and a lifetime of behavioral 



24 



Nutrition in Crisis 



conditioning telling us to eat anything that tastes good—but 

even those who fall off the wagon will usually return to a low- 

carbohydrate diet. Nutritionists will tell you that u yo-yo dieting” 

has some risk, but there is no evidence for that, and most of us 

are happy for any period where we are thin, however long it lasts. 


Whatever your concerns about low-carbohydrate, for most 

people, low-fat diets are even worse than doing nothing. The 

hunger will simply be too much to deal with. Sure, if you can 

get yourself into the frame of mind where you like the “lean 

and hungry” feeling, then anything that reduces calories will be 

okay. However, most people arerit able to deal with that feeling 

and will not be able to lose any significant amount of weight on 

a low-fat diet. 


The scientific literature backs up the anecdotal evidence 

in support of low-carbohydrate. Jeff Volek, one of the major 

researchers in carbohydrate restriction, put this spin on it: 

“Nutrition research is hard. Too many things change and its 

easy to come up with nothing. When you study low-carb diets, 

people lose weight. Put people on a low-carb diet and you get 

real data.” As I finish this hook, the long-standing refusal of 

the nutritional establishment to face the data is finally falling 

away. It will be increasingly easy to follow" a low-carbohydrate 

approach and to have the support of a physician in doing so. 

Experimentally, carbohydrate restriction has better compliance 

than anything else because it gives immediate results, possibly 

due to the greater satiety of protein and fat, or, more likely, 

due to the poor satiety of most carbohydrates. You can test for 

yourself, at least at the level of perception. 


Later chapters will describe experimental studies that 

support these conclusions about low-carbohydrate strategies, 

but there is one truly remarkable phenomenon that tells you 

about the edge low-carbohydrate diets have in satiety When 

diet comparisons are carried out experimentally, most often the 



Handling the Crisis 



25 



protocol is to allow the low-carbohydrate group the freedom to 

eat ad lib as long as they follow the restrictions on carbohydrate. 

Low-fat diets, on the other hand, are restricted to a fixed number 

of calories. The reason that the Atkins diet and other low- 

carbohydrate approaches put no limit on consumption is that 

fat and protein intake is self-limiting when carbohydrate is low. 

Setting up the experiment this way, however, constitutes poor 

experimental design because you are now testing two things; 

the ability of a low-carbohydrate diet to limit caloric intake as 

well as the proposed difference in physiologic effects of the type 

of diet—the metabolic advantage. The low-carbohydrate diet 

almost always wins in such comparisons, but because of the poor 

experimental design, you can t see how much of the success can 

be attributed to the greater satiety of low-carbohydrate diets as 

opposed to the greater weight loss per calorie. The results do tell 

you one thing, though: As advertised, you dont have to count 

calories in a diet based on carbohydrate restriction. 


Perhaps the greatest virtue of carbohydrate restriction rests 

with its fail-safe feature. If you are not rapidly losing weight, 

or if you seem to hit the wall, it is a way of eating that gives 

you freedom from the sense of fighting a war against fat. You 

will almost never gain weight and you will escape that over¬ 

bearing feeling that every meal is a battle. This feeling might 

actually be the greatest threat of obesity. As a threat to health, 

excess weight is probably exaggerated. Mortality correlates with 

weight only at the very extremes. The major threat is the sense 

of loss of control. I am not a health care provider, but I get 

emails from executives, officers in the military, and other people 

who hold dominion over their professional world and who have 

trouble controlling their own body mass. The struggle can be a 

tremendous burden and can take over entire lives. Cut out most 

of the carbohydrates in your life, and you stand a chance of 

getting rid of the burden. 



26 



Nutrition in Crisis 



The best exercise, like the best diet, is the one that you can get yourself to 

do regularly. For that reason, exercise is highly individualized. Proponents 

of each type of exercise think that the others are not good, but the impor¬ 

tant thing is to find one that fits your style, or, more important, one that 

you get in the habit of doing. I am personally a believer in slow burn (slow 

repetition with heavy weights). This type of exercise is probably best for 

people who think that they are entering old age, in that it is efficient: You 

can get perceptible benefit from one set of ninety seconds. 1 


An encouraging development in exercise physiology is the technique 

called periodization, which is really just a pretentious way of saying that 

it is good to mix up different types of exercise on different days. You now 

have official sanction for switching between different types of exercise 

because you are bored. 



Doing It: 


Prepare for Battle, Prepare for Victory 


Even the easiest diet has problems, ups and downs, and temptations. You 

have to plan out how you are going to handle different situations. If you 

know that the celebratory business meeting will be serving pizza, go in 

with a plan (eat beforehand, decide if you are comfortable enough to eat 

the toppings without the crust, etc.) There are techniques for staying 

on your diet and dealing with hunger, but you also have to prepare for 

success—that is, what to do if you actually aren’t hungry. Suppose you’ve 

started a low-carbohydrate diet, you sit down to the recommended broc¬ 

coli and steak, and you are full after eating one quarter of what you usually 

eat. You should be prepared to stop. In restaurants, doggie-bags are a good 

strategy unless you’re on a first date, but then you’ll probably be on your 

best behavior anyway. What should you do if the birthday cake looks 

disgusting and you don’t want to eat it? It is, after all, your boss s birthday. 

There are many techniques. Put the birthday cake on your plate and walk 

around the room stabbing at it with your fork, for example. After a while 

you can set it on the sideboard with the other half-eaten pieces of cake. 

If you are worried about wasting food, ask for a doggie bag—tell them, 

“this is so good, but I can’t eat another bite”—and then compost it when 

you return home. 



Handling the Crisis 



27 



Doing It: 


Minister to Yourself, Cross-Examine the Experts 


Doctor: Therein the patient 


Must minister to himself. 


Macbeth: Throw physic [medicine] to the dogs; Til none of it . 


—William Shakespeare, Macbeth 


Doctors dont study nutrition. Nutritionists dont study medicine. Neither 

studies much science. This is an exaggeration, of course—many great scien¬ 

tists were trained as MDs—but it is not without some truth. Having an 

MD does not qualify a doctor on anything except their medical specialty. 

Experts have the obligation to justify their opinions, and you have the right 

to expect that justification. In the end, though, you are your own expert. 

Strive to learn as much as you can, and go for results rather than experts. 


Perhaps most troubling is the proposition that the whole field of profes¬ 

sional nutrition is fundamentally flawed. It is genuinely hard to understand 

how the progression of warnings about the lethal health effects of red 

meat or white rice can continue. These conclusions are not only contrary 

to our own intuitions, but when you look at the underlying data, they are 

actually scientifically meaningless. It is difficult to accept that the dozens 

of publications coming out of the Harvard School of Public Health and 

published in The New England Journal of Medicine are of extremely poor 

scientific quality. Where s the peer review? Where’s the expert training? 

The failure of experts and the lack of genuine peer review represent a major 

component of the crisis in nutrition and in medicine at large. In this book 

I will give you the tools to see how establishment medicine went off track, 

and I will show you how the three rules will help you minister to yourself. 



Doing It: The Low-Carbohydrate Principles 


My personal preference is for principles over formal diets. However, if you 

like diets with precise instructions, there are millions. For low-carbohydrate 

and ketogenic diets, The New Atkins for a New You , 2 Protein Power [ and 

The Art and Science of Low-Carbohydrate Living are good places to start. 

Paleo diets might also be of interest—they are like the Mediterranean diet 



28 



Nutrition in Crisis 



in that nobody really knows what they are, but they are generally low- 

carbohydrate and employ a higher ratio of science to emotion than other 

options. Numerous books and websites will give you precise instructions 

and recipes for any of these diets. If your MO is to just fit the general 

low-carbohydrate strategy to your own lifestyle, on the other hand, these 

are the big principles. 


Your Carbs Come from Vegetables 


The simplest way to break into carbohydrate restriction is by brute force: 

no rice, no potatoes, no bread, no pasta, and no dessert beyond a small 

amount of fruit. What you have to sacrifice is dependent on your current 

diet. If you normally eat a steak with potatoes and broccoli, leave out the 

potatoes. This might be all you have to do. If you are full from this smaller 

amount of food, put the rest in the refrigerator, or compost it if its going 

bad. If you still have an appetite, you can have more steak, but in fact most 

people don’t want more steak even if they can afford it—so it’s usually 

more broccoli. Vegetables contain some carbohydrate, but the important 

thing is that you don’t really have to count anything. You are likely to have 

real success with this simple approach. If you have diabetes or metabolic 

syndrome, you are virtually guaranteed to get better—although, again, if 

you are taking medication, you have to do this with a physician who can 

lower your medications appropriately. 


To delve a bit deeper, you need to understand a couple of things: First, 

there is a graded response, which means that the benefit is roughly propor¬ 

tional to the amount of carbohydrate that you remove from your eating; 

and second, there is a breakpoint, where there is enhanced weight loss. 

The breakpoint is generally marked by the presence of ketone bodies in 

the blood (ketosis) and urine. Strictly speaking, the presence of ketone 

bodies in the urine is called ketonuria (although frequently taken as a sign 

of ketosis). The ketone bodies indicate a significant switch from reliance 

on carbohydrate as an energy source to a new, predominantly fat-based 

metabolism. At this point, you will have to attend more precisely to how 

much carbohydrate you actually ingest. The simple rules above will likely 

get you in the carbohydrate range of 100 grams per day, which is a big 

switch for most Americans. To go into ketosis, you will have to go below 

20-50 grams per day, though different people have different cutoffs. 



Handling the Crisis 



29 



Though the Atkins diet has assumed the status of Kleenex and is 

considered the generic low-carbohydrate diet, it actually has a more 

specific strategy than you might realize. The Atkins diet’s recommenda¬ 

tion is to achieve ketosis for two weeks, followed by gradually re-adding 

carbohydrate into the diet, presumably for reasons of taste or possibly on 

the principle that you want to eat similarly to how you used to but with¬ 

out making yourself fat. Many people stay in ketosis indefinitely, but this 

requires more attention and usually a period of adaptation. 


My survey of an online support group called the Low-Carber Forums 5 

found that the majority of people on low-carbohydrate diets eat all the 

nonroot vegetables they want without counting grams of carbohydrate, 

even though most of these individuals thought that they were on the 

Atkins diet, which specifies precise grams of carbohydrate. 


If you eat at home a lot, it is important to learn how to cook vegetables. 

Like many tasks, cooking vegetables involves spending a great deal of time 

thinking and procrastinating and a relatively small amount of time actually 

doing the job. A good solution is to time yourself—the task likely ends up 

taking less time than you’d imagined. So, find the appropriate vegetable 

dishes for either quick cooking or for cooking in advance. Cauliflower, for 

example, is a common component of low-carbohydrate diets, and you can 

make steamed cauliflower in just a few minutes. Eat it straight or use it 

later for other very quick recipes. 


Don’t Be Afraid of Fat 


People have always eaten fat. The antifat campaign is of recent origin, and 

countless histories and exposes have proven its lack of grounding in scien¬ 

tific evidence. It has been one of the truly bizarre phenomena in the history 

of science—in this most scientific of periods, we have simply ignored the 

failures of the numerous experimental trials of the low-fat idea. The big 

clinical studies—the Framingham Study, the Oslo Diet Heart Study, and 

a dozen others culminating in the Womens Health Initiative—have found 

no support for the theory. When pressed, most health agencies say that 

there is nothing wrong with fat in general, only with saturated fat, but even 

the evidence for removing saturated fat is missing from the big studies. 


From the scientific point of view, however, these studies were successful. 

They asked the real question: “Are low-fat, especially low-saturated-fat, 




30 



Nutrition in Crisis 



dietary patterns beneficial for prevention of cardiovascular disease and 

other health problems?” Many were well done, within the limitations of 

large population studies, and they gave a clear answer: No. Low-fat diets, 

especially low-saturated-fat dietary patterns, do not provide benefit. A 

clear question was posed and the answer was equally clear. Of course, as 

in court, you cannot be found innocent, only not guilty. There might well 

be conditions where saturated fat is a risk, but we have not found them. 

We suspect that high saturated fat in combination with high carbohydrate 

might have risk but we haven't even been able to show that. 


So, my advice—grounded in the existing evidence—is to not be afraid 

of fat. If you are concerned about saturated fat, there are numerous 

alternatives, but I would not advise trying to go low-carbohydrate and 

low-fat unless you like being hungry. You might, in fact, like the feel¬ 

ing—feelings are different if we impose them ourselves rather than having 

them imposed on us. Total calorie restriction does have general health 

benefits, but, even there, the de facto reduction in carbohydrate is likely 

the controlling factor. 


Desserts and Sweets 


Carbohydrates as a chemical class encompass simple sugars and their 

polymers, starch and related compounds. Reducing sugar intake is part 

of reducing carbohydrate intake, and sugar is an easier target for elimina¬ 

tion than other sources of carbohydrate: Candy is considered frivolous. 

Because sugar assumes a more discretionary position, it might be the best 

place to start reducing carbohydrate intake, and for some people, elimina¬ 

tion of sugar might be all that's necessary. It's important that you don't 

put starch back in place of the sugar that you remove, though. What the 

strange collection of bedfellows currently involved in the political move¬ 

ment that I call fructophobia, the attack on sugar, forgot to tell you is that 

sugar is a carbohydrate. If you cut out sugar and replace it with “healthy” 

high-grain, high-carbohydrate oatmeal, you are stacking the cards against 

yourself (even if you think that oatmeal offers benefits in fiber that offset 

the number of carbohydrates). Many people, anecdotally, gain benefit by 

simply removing sugar-sweetened soda, but if you have a sweet tooth, you 

might need some help. One strategy is to imagine that you are conducting 

an experiment. The hypothesis from anecdotal observation is that cravings 



Handling the Crisis 



31 



for sweets disappear after three days without sweets. Your experiment will 

test whether that is true for you. 


If you must have something sweet, there are several nonnutritive sweet¬ 

eners—some natural, some artificial. You might want to avoid artificial 

sweeteners, however, especially those in diet soda, because they can sustain 

your taste for sweets and provoke bad reactions in certain individuals. The 

scare stories about the artificial sweeteners are not scientifically sound, but 

the possible psychological effect of “sweetness” might be real. 


You should do whatever works for you, but I think that for most people, 

it is best to avoid the common nutritional advice that you are allowed treats 

or that you are allowed a cheat day. You are not allowed treats. Nobodys 

perfect and sweet things do taste good, and you will have some. We all 

screw up periodically, and when this happens, you just have to get back to 

the plan. However, sweets are not allowed in the sense of being a specific 

feature of your diet. Consumed sweets are, scientifically speaking, experi¬ 

mental error. There are people who are able to incorporate some sweets as 

part of their diet, who don't deviate from the single ice-cream bar that they 

have every day as their lone large source of carbs. Some people can get 

away with eating these sweets, and if you like food, you will certainly find 

foods that are worth the risk to your diet, but they are not allowed in the 

sense of a recommendation. There is a big difference between the attitude 

that you cant be perfect and the attitude that you deserve an occasional 

treat. But I am not suggesting that you should indulge feelings of guilt or 

mentally beat yourself up. The psychologist Alfred Adler always advised: 

“Do the wrong thing or feel guilty but not both.” 


The current hysteria about sugar might help you cut back, but there 

is great danger in characterizing things as forbidden fruit, as our very 

earliest history shows, especially when someone is trying to ban that 

forbidden fruit. Being told that you cant have something might make 

it more appealing, and when you see the absurd lengths that the media 

and researchers alike go to in order to demonize sugar, you might begin 

to think it is safe. Lawyers call this the Reverse Mussolini Fallacy: Just’ 

because Mussolini made the trains run on time doesn't mean you want 

them to be late. (The last time I was in Italy, the trains did run on time, 

contrary to the Italian stereotype.) Just because the USDA says it's bad, 

doesn't mean it's good. 



32 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Fruit and Other Tricky Foods 


All fruits contain sugar, and as with any type of food, it is best to stay with 

the rule: Eat to the meter. If it doesn’t interfere with weight loss or blood 

sugar, or whatever your goal is, then it is OK. I like Suzanne Somers’s 

technique. Unlike many experts who might be thin themselves (what does 

Walter Willett know about fighting fat?), Somers lost a part in Starsky and 

Hutch because they told her she was “a little too chunky.” She recommends 

eating fruit in pieces—half an apple now, the other half later—with the 

goal of avoiding insulin spikes. 6 


Mike and Mary Dan Eades, authors of Protein Power, ran a clinic for 

many years. They had thousands of patients on low-carbohydrate diets. 

Despite generally achieving great success, they had several patients who 

complained that they had faithfully adhered to the diet but were not losing 

weight. The Eades found that the three most common foods that caused 

trouble were cheese, nuts, and nut butters. When these were reduced or 

removed from the (already low-carbohydrate) diet, patients were able to 

continue to lose weight. Cheese is probably a simple matter of overcon¬ 

sumption. Although apparently safe, moderation must be applied to cheese. 

Dr. Atkins recommended that you restrict yourself to hard cheeses, 7 which 

pretty much means serious cheeses that you might find in a gourmet cheese 

shop. Although it’s not obvious what was behind his recommendation, the 

greater intensity might be more satisfying, and the current price of good 

cheese (e.g., two- or three-year aged Gouda) also ensures moderation for 

most of us. 


“Not a License to Gorge” 


The phrase “not a license to gorge” appears in the original Atkins book 

several times. 8 The idea is that, although there are no stated limits on what 

you can eat as long as you keep carbohydrate low, overeating is not encour¬ 

aged. The principle that “a calorie is a calorie” is not correct, but calories 

do count, and if you are concerned about your weight, you undoubtedly 

already know that it is possible to defeat any diet. The nutritionists usually 

give you exactly the wrong advice but are actually right about eating slowly. 

Satiety sets in slowly. For people who like food, the prescription is simply to 

not eat if you are not hungry. A possible exception is a situation where you 

feel that you have the kind of cravings that will set your low-carbohydrate 



Handling the Crisis 



33 



diet back. In such a case it might be useful to eat something thats allowed 

to satiate the craving, even if it seems like overeating: some high-protein, 

high-fat food (if you have an Eastern European butcher, real kielbasa is 

best). Eventually, your cravings will stabilize. In general, the corollary to 

the second part of Rule 2 is that if you have to overeat, don’t overeat carbs. 



Diet Definitions 


There are many variations of diets based on carbohydrate restriction. As 

a treatment for diabetes, the principle is to keep carbohydrate as low 

as possible. In less stringent conditions, there might be more room for 



Table 1.1. Operational Definitions of Carbohydrate-Restricted Diets 



Diet 


Carbohydrate restriction 


Very low- 

carbohydrate 

ketogenic diet 

(VLCKD) 


20-50 grams per day, or less than 10% of the 2,000 

kilocalories per day diet. Generally, although not always, 

accompanied by ketosis, this is the level of the early phases 

of the plans in many popular carbohydrate books. 


Low- 


carbohydrate diet 


Less than 130 grams per day, or less than 26% of a nominal 

2,000 kilocalories per day diet. This corresponds to the ADA 

definition of 130 grams per day. This is a generally accepted 

number, likely derived from misinterpretation of Cahill’s study 

of the onset of ketosis. 


Moderate- 

carbohydrate diet 


26-45% of the 2,000 kilocalories per day diet. The upper limit 

is chosen as the approximate carbohydrate intake before the 

obesity epidemic (43%). Current consumption is about 49%. 


High- 


carbohydrate diet 


Greater than 45% of the 2,000 kilocalories per day diet. 

Recommended target on ADA websites. The 2015 Dietary 

Guidelines for Americans recommend 45-65% carbohydrate. 


For comparison 



Pre-obesity 

epidemic 

(1971-1974- 

NHANES 1) 


Men: 42% carbohydrate 


(-250 grams for 2,450 kilocalories per day) 


Women: 45% carbohydrate 


(-150 grams for 1,550 kilocalories per day] 


Year 1999-2000 


Men: 49% carbohydrate 


(-330 grams for 2,600 kilocalories per day) 


Women: 52% carbohydrate 


(-230 grams for 1,900 kilocalories per day] 
















34 



Nutrition in Crisis 



maneuvering. If specific diets are referred to, table 1.1 provides the associ¬ 

ated guidelines that have been published in several peer-reviewed journals 

by professionals with the credentials and experience. 


These definitions are important. Authors in the nutritional literature give 

themselves license to call anything that they want a low-carbohydrate diet. 

With a straw man in hand, it is not hard to show that a low-carbohydrate 

diet is dangerous to your health, but the results have very little to do with 

the actual established guidelines. 


Depending on your goal and who you are, you might experience a 

graded response: The greater the carbohydrate reduction, the greater the 

weight loss, and the greater the improvement in blood glucose. Particularly 

in weight loss, however, there might be a threshold effect. The onset of 

ketosis, which for most people occurs at a daily intake of about 30 grams, 

will have a more dramatic effect. 



-CHAPTER 2 - 



Whaddaya Know? 



F or several years, we gave incoming medical students a questionnaire 

to assess their knowledge of nutrition. These students were among 

the most accomplished in the country, but, like everybody else, 

most of their nutritional information came from rumors and the fronts of 

packages on supermarket shelves. We didn’t keep any quantitative feed¬ 

back on the quiz, but some of students liked the format, and you might, 

too. No particular knowledge is assumed beyond your life experience as 

a nutritional end user, an eater. The quiz only tests how you were able 

to sift through all the information thats available on the internet and in 

popular books, and it is intended as a teaching device: The answers provide 

basic information. I provide the quiz first so you can see how you do, and 

then, as I go through the answers, I will delve into the nutritional concepts 

associated with each question. So, whaddaya know? 


1. The most energy-dense food (most calories per gram) is: 


□ carbohydrate □ protein 


□ fat □ alcohol 


2. For a slice of buttered bread, which is more fattening? 


□ the butter □ You cannot tell from the 


□ the bread information given. 


□ Both are equally fattening. 


3. During the epidemic of obesity and diabetes, the macronutrient 

that increased most was: 


□ carbohydrate □ All were about the 


□ protein same. Calories increased 


□ fat across the board. 



36 



Nutrition in Crisis 



4. The macronutrient most likely to 


raise blood glucose in people with 


type 2 diabetes is: 



□ carbohydrate 


□ fat 


□ protein 


□ alcohol 


5. The dietary requirement for carbohydrate is: 


□ approximately 


□ as much as possible 


130 grams per day 


□ There is no dietary 


□ approximately 


requirement for 


50 percent of calories 


carbohydrate. 


6. The amount of carbohydrate recommended by the American 


Diabetes Association and other health agencies is: 


□ approximately 


□ as much 


130 grams per day 


as possible 


□ approximately 


□ as little 


50 percent of calories 


as possible 


7. A good source of monounsaturated fat is: (check all that apply) 



□ butter □ avocado oil □ flaxseed oil 


□ olive oil □ corn oil 


□ canola oil □ soybean oil 


8. The diet component that is most likely to raise triglycerides 

(fat in the blood) is: 


□ fat □ carbohydrate □ protein 


9. In general, what effect does a low-fat diet have on HDL-C 

(high-density lipoprotein cholesterol, i.e., “good cholesterol”)? 


□ increase □ decrease □ no change 


10. The dietary change that is most likely to increase the risk of 

cardiovascular disease is: 


□ unsaturated fat □ unsaturated fat □ carbohydrate 


-> saturated fat -> carbohydrate unsaturated fat 



Whaddaya Know? 



37 



□ carbohydrate 

saturated fat 



□ saturated fat 

-> carbohydrate 



□ saturated fat 

unsaturated fat 



The Calorie, the Calorimeter 


Now that youVe taken the time to complete the quiz, let’s see how you 

actually did. I encourage you to keep a tally as you go along and see how 

you stack up against the incoming medical students. 


1. The most energy-dense food 

(most calories per gram) is: 


□ carbohydrate 

[El fat 


□ protein 


□ alcohol 


Our first-year medical students do surprisingly poorly on the first 

question, considering how basic it is. Typically only 85 percent of our 

incoming class gets it right. Although they have not yet been through 

the metabolism course, this is a highly educated group of people, so you 

would assume that everyone knows that fat is the most energy-dense 

macronutrient. The likely explanation is that they are not curious about 

nutrition because they don't see it as part of medicine and because they are 

mostly young, healthy, and thin. 


The operational numbers in kilocalories per gram are 4, 4, 9, and 7 for 

carbohydrate, protein, fat, and alcohol, respectively. Calories are a measure 

of energy, and calories in nutrition represent the energy that we can obtain 

by metabolizing ingested food. In physics, energy is defined as the ability 

to do work, which is not so different from the common day-to-day idea. By 

using a device called a calorimeter to measure heat—one form of energy— 

we are able to determine the total number of calories in a sample of food. 

The food is placed in a small container in an atmosphere of oxygen under 

pressure, and it is then ignited. The container is surrounded by a water 

bath that heats up when the sample is ignited. By recording the change 

in temperature in the water bath, we can calculate the amount of heat 



Student response (%) 


11 


85 





38 



Nutrition in Crisis 



generated from the combustion, and this number can be assigned to the 

oxidation of the food. This is the real definition of the nutritional calorie. 


In physics, the definition of a calorie is given as the amount of heat 

required to raise the temperature of water by one degree. The dietary “calo¬ 

rie” is equal to a physical kilocalorie (kcal)—that is, 1,000 physical calories. 

Use of “kcal” in nutrition is increasing, and in this book we use kcal when 

referring to quantity for the sake of clarity This is one small step toward 

helping nutrition become scientific. 


The definition I have given for a calorie is quite simple, but there are 

some very important nuances: The calories assigned to a food represent the 

energy for complete combustion of that food in oxygen. Calories refer to 

the following chemical reaction, not to the food itself: 


Food + Oxygen -> Carbon Dioxide + Water 

or 


Food + O 2 CO 2 + H 2 O 


Heat produced in the calorimeter measures the energy for this specific 

reaction. Again, the energy is in the reaction, not in the food. It is not like 

particle physics where the mass of a particle is given in units of electron- 

volts, a measure of energy, (because of E = me 2 ). We will come back to this 

point when we delve into thermodynamics and consider what is wrong 

with the idea that a “calorie is a calorie” in chapter 9. 


Fat is the most energy-dense macronutrient at 9 kcal per gram (keal/g), 

and for that reason, it is considered inherently fattening by nutritionists. 

This is the basis of traditional recommendations for low-fat diets for 

obesity. There is more to the problem than caloric density, however, and it 

turns out that there is more to getting fat than total calories. To see how 

this all plays out in a real situation, consider the next question. 


2. For a slice of buttered bread, 


which is more fattening? Student response (%) 


□ The butter. 11 


□ The bread. 85 


□ Both are equally fattening. 4 


[xj You cannot tell from the information given. 0 



Whaddaya Know? 



39 



While we do not normally ask trick questions, this falls into that 

category. An argument could be made that because butter has an energy 

density of 9 kcal/g, it is inherently more fattening, but you really need 

more information. If you put a lot of butter on the bread, it would indeed 

be more fattening, but in fact, people rarely put more than one tablespoon 

of butter (approximately 100 kcal) on a slice of bread (also approximately 

100 kcal). Yes, fat is the most calorically dense food, but caloric density, like 

any density, can be very misleading: Density is a measurement per amount 

of material—per unit of volume, per gram, per something—so it matters 

how much of that something you actually have. Calories per gram is not 

informative unless you know how many grams. Things like caloric density, 

or any density for that matter, are measures of intensity, and technically are 

called intensive variables. An intensive variable does not depend on how 

much you have: One tablespoon of butter has the same energy density 

(kcal/g) as two tablespoons of butter, but obviously two tablespoons have 

twice the total calories. Such total, absolute measurements are called exten¬ 

sive variables. It ? s like that riddle: Which is heavier? A pound of uranium 

or a pound of Styrofoam? Of course they are the same. A pound is a pound. 

Uranium has an extremely high density (about 1.6 times that of lead), so 

a pound of uranium is only a little bigger than a major league baseball, 

whereas a pound of Styrofoam would fill up an entire room. 


Fraction, or percent, is another kind of measure of intensity that can be 

misleading. Looking ahead, this is one reason that providing the percent 

increase of risk in a clinical test is not helpful, however much the media 

and scientific literature might seize on such numbers. After all, your odds 

of winning the lottery are increased by 100 percent, or doubled, by buying 

two tickets instead of one—but should that really make you want to play? 

Bottom line: When you hear people say that fat has more calories per 

gram, know that it is irrelevant. You have to know how many grams you 

are actually eating. 



The Obesity Epidemic 


So, what did we eat during the obesity epidemic of the past thirty or forty 

years? What kinds of macronutrients were in our diet? You might know 

this better than our students, who probably did not attend to the problem. 



40 



Nutrition in Crisis 



3. During the epidemic of obesity and diabetes, 


the macronutrient that increased most was: Student response (%) 


0 carbohydrate 53 


□ protein 4 


□ fat 2 


□ All were about the same. 41 


Calories increased across the board. 


At this point many students have caught on to where I’m coming from, 

and there are probably more votes for carbohydrate than if this had been 

the first question. However, many people, students included, still think that 

calories increased across the board. The epidemic of obesity and diabetes 

has been accompanied by a substantial decrease in the percentage of fat in 

peoples* diets, and, at least for men, the absolute amount of fat consumed 

also went down slightly. 


Figure 2.1 shows data from the National Health and Nutrition 

Examination Survey (NHANES), which was conducted at the intervals 

listed on the horizontal axis. The vertical axis is the absolute amount of 

calories consumed, and the large increase in energy consumed appears to be 

due entirely to a dramatic increase in the consumption of carbohydrate. The 

percent change shown along the top indicates the expected decrease in fat, 

but, at least for men, the absolute amount of fat (total calories) and, notably, 

the absolute amount of saturated fat went down. (The extent to which this 

gender difference plays out in real life is not clear. Anecdotally, it is easier 

for men to lose weight, but the major effects of metabolism are qualitatively 

the same across sexes, and the benefit of controlling the glucose—insulin axis 

applies to women as well. Obviously, other hormones might play a role.) 


There remains a lot of error in these kinds of surveys, but it is quite clear 

that there was no increase in fat intake. The clearest conclusion is that 

an increase in carbohydrate consumption, rather than fat consumption, is 

associated with greater total intake, and that a “Western diet,” as they call 

it in the nutrition literature, does not mean a high-fat diet. It is widely 

said that association does not imply causality. The more accurate state¬ 

ment is that association does not necessarily imply causality. Few people 

would deny that the association of dietary calories and body mass is causal, 

however nonlinear the association might be. Whether the association 



Whaddaya Know? 



41 



3500 

3000 

15 2500 



% 2000 


(D 



> 1500 


I .000 


500 




NHANES Men 


CHO:FAT:PROT 


42:37:17 40:33:16 



Overall intake 



+ 6 . 8 % 



Carbohydrate 


Fat 


Saturated fat 


1971-74 [I) 1988-94 (III) 1976-80 [II] 


Years 



+23.4% 


-5% 


-14% 


1999-2000 



3500 



3000 



= 2500 


(U 



© 2000 


id 



>. 1500 


D) 


k_ 


0) 


w 1000 



500 





NHANES Women 


CHO:FAT:PROT 


45:36:17 


20 


15 


10 



70 75 '80 '85 '90 '95 '00 


Years 


Overall intake 




52:33:15 



+21.7% 



Carbohydrate 


Fat 


Saturated fat 


1971-74 [I] 1988-94 (III) 1976-80 


Years 



+38.4% 


+ 11 % 


— +3% 

1999-2000 



Figure 2.1. Consumption of macronutrients during the epidemic of obesity and 

diabetes. The horizontal axis represents the periods in which data was collected. The 

left vertical axis is the absolute energy input in kilocalories. The numbers on the far 

right refer to the percent change in calories. The ratios of macronutrients are shown 

along the top. Note: CHO = Carbohydrate. Data from the National Health and Nutritional 

Examination Survey. 





42 



Nutrition in Crisis 



between increased carbohydrate intake and increased calories is causative 

is one of the central questions explored in this book. The argument will 

be that, given the effectiveness of low-carbohydrate, high-fat diets as a 

treatment and sometimes a virtual cure for diabetes, it would be surprising 

if carbohydrate was not involved in some causative role. 


Finally, there is an obvious association between the official advice of 

the USDA, the AHA, and other authorities to reduce fat and increase 

carbohydrates, and what people actually did. They reduced fat, at least as a 

percentage of calories, and they dramatically increased carbohydrate. 


Looking ahead, one way to test whether there is a causal link between 

carbohydrate intake and obesity is to simply reduce carbohydrate under 

conditions of fixed calories while monitoring weight and incidence of 

diabetes. There are some good experiments that test this (see descriptions 

ofVolek’s experiment in chapters 7 and 11). Whatever else can be drawn 

from the NHANES data, the association between increased carbohydrate/ 

decreased fat intake and obesity and diabetes is the single result that makes 

the largest impact on our medical students, and it remains an undercurrent 

in any analysis of the role of macronutrients. 


4. The macronutrient most likely to raise blood 


glucose in people with type 2 diabetes is: Student response (%) 


(El carbohydrate 83 


□ protein Not available 


□ fat Not available 


□ alcohol Not available 


This question is, or should be, obvious. The correct answer was chosen 

by 83 percent of our students. Statistics on the wrong answers were lost, 

but the surprise is that anybody got it wrong. Diabetes is fundamentally a 

disease—or, really, several diseases—of carbohydrate intolerance. Glucose 

normally stimulates secretion of the hormone insulin from beta cells of the 

pancreas. Insulin is a kind of master hormone, controlling fat and protein 

metabolism as well as maintaining normal blood glucose, and in that 

sense, carbohydrate controls other aspects of metabolism. People with type 

1 diabetes cannot produce insulin in response to blood glucose. People 

with type 2 diabetes have progressive deterioration of the beta cells. They 



Whaddaya Know? 



43 



do produce insulin but their cells respond poorly—a phenomenon called 

insulin resistance. Diabetes is as much a disease of fat metabolism as of 

carbohydrate metabolism: The primary effect of insulin is on synthesis and 

breakdown of fat, and a person with type 2 diabetes might have excessive 

fatty acids in their blood. Nonetheless, the most obvious characteristic and 

the major cause of other symptoms remains the hyperglycemia (high blood 

glucose). Different carbohydrate-containing foods raise blood glucose to 

different extents, but the general principle holds that carbohydrate is the 

primary influence on blood glucose in people with diabetes. 



The Dietary Requirement for Carbohydrate 


There is no requirement for dietary carbohydrate as there is for the so-called 

essential amino acids or essential fatty acids. This does not mean that I 

recommend doing without them altogether, even if this were possible (it 

isn't: even meat has a small amount of carbohydrate). 


5. The dietary requirement for carbohydrate is: Student response (%) 



□ approximately 130 grams per day 


22 


□ approximately 50 percent of calories 


32 


□ as much as possible 



IE] There is no dietary 


26 


requirement for carbohydrate. 




The fact that there is no dietary requirement for carbohydrate means, 

in a practical sense, that if you do want to reduce carbohydrate, there is 

no biological limit on how much you can restrict the intake. The extent 

to which you actually do restrict carbohydrate intake will depend on your 

personal reaction and taste, but you do not need to consume any at all. 

Nutritionists will emphasize that the brain needs glucose, but your body 

is capable of making glucose from protein through the process known as 

gluconeogenesis and supplying glucose from storage as glycogen. There 

are also alternative fuels available in the form of ketone bodies. If you did 

need dietary carbohydrate, you would die if you went without food for a 

week—after all, you store a lot of fat but not much carbohydrate. I write 

more on glycogen and gluconeogenesis in chapter 5. 



44 



Nutrition in Crisis 



6. The amount of carbohydrate recommended 

by the American Diabetes Association and 

other health agencies is: 


□ approximately 130 grams per day 


M approximately 50 percent of calories 


□ as much as possible 


□ as little as possible 



Student response (%) 


33 


35 



30 



It is hard to believe that a diabetes agency would recommend any 

significant amount of carbohydrate, yet their 2008 dietary guidelines 

contain the rather remarkable advice that “Sucrose-containing foods can 

be substituted for other carbohydrates in the meal plan or, if added to the 

meal plan, covered with insulin or other glucose-lowering medications. 

Care should be taken to avoid excess energy intake. (A)” 1 


The truth is that starch, which is a polymer of glucose, is undoubtedly 

worse for people with diabetes than sugar. (Sugar is half glucose and half 

fructose, and fructose does not stimulate insulin release.) It is the phrase 

“added to the meal plan,” and the advice that follows, that is most jarring. 

To many people it would seem that the ADA is saying that it is okay to 

make things worse as long as you take more drugs. The “(A)” mark indi¬ 

cates that they consider this advice to be their highest level of evidence. 

They don't cite that evidence, but it is surely not experimental. While this 

book was being written, the ADA quietly dropped this passage from their 

2013 guidelines, 2 but they have not explicitly stated that it was wrong. It 

is unknown whether the rank and file of ADA membership ever read the 

organizations follow-up statements, and if so, whether they thought they 

were of high quality or simply political statements that no one had time 

to fight. One of the organizations statements from 2012 reads as follows: 

“Although brain fuel needs can be met on lower-carbohydrate diets, long¬ 

term metabolic effects of very low-carbohydrate diets are unclear.” 3 


In fact, the long-term effects of low-carbohydrate diets are clear. Very 

clear.There are trials spanning one or two years, and internet sites and forums 

make apparent that low-carbohydrate is a way of life for many people with 

diabetes. Although personal stories are hard to document, we would have 

heard if there were any indication of long-term problems. The medical 

establishment has been obsessed with finding fault with low-carbohydrate 



Whaddaya Know? 



45 



diets, and over forty years, they have found nothing. Zero. Zilch. Zip. More 

important, there is no reason to suggest that there would be any long-term 

effects. In science, you don’t start from scratch. You don’t assume that there 

is harm unless there is a reason to. Nothing about reducing carbohydrate 

suggests harm. The ADAs use of the word unclear implies conflicting data, 

but there are no conflicting data and there is no reason to expect any. I 

suggested to a spokesperson for the ADA that the organization was stron¬ 

ger on what they were opposed to than on anything positive they had to 

offer. She admitted that it was a fair criticism. So, why are they opposed 

to carbohydrate restriction? The ADA guidelines of 2010 say: “Such diets 

eliminate many foods that are important sources of energy, fiber, vitamins, 

and minerals and are important in dietary palatability.” 4 


If “care should be taken to avoid excess energy intake,” then why would 

you need carbohydrate as an “an important source of energy?” Diabetes is a 

severe disease—does anyone think that taking extra vitamins and minerals is 

as important to a person with diabetes as maintaining healthy blood glucose? 

There is also a subtle switch from using the word “carbohydrate” to refer to 

the micronutrient, to using it to mean carbohydrate-containing food. Finally, 

I would suggest that dietary palatability is not the ADA’s area of expertise. 


The guidelines from the ADA, as from other health agencies, are supposed 

to be serious, but, in fact, have the character of an infomercial. Their stan¬ 

dards are those of selling a product rather than presenting a scientific case. 

It seems that we are supposed to just take what they say at face value. The 

ADA is a private organization, but they receive public support, at least in 

that they are tax-exempt. Their experts are often federally funded as well. 

Are they really free to say whatever they want without justification? This is 

the kernel of the crisis in nutrition. Science is still continuous with common 

sense. It is not okay at all to, as the ADA says, eat more carbohydrate if 

“covered with insulin or other glucose-lowering medications.” If you can 

take less medication, that is considered a benefit in every disease I know. 



Lipid Chemistry 


The second half of the quiz is about lipids. The discussion of what to eat 

frequently boils down to how much fat one should consume. The medi¬ 

cal establishment—including the AHA, ADA, Harvard School of Public 



46 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Glycemic Index 


Politically Correct Low-Carbohydrate 


The original questionnaire given to medical students asked one 

question about the glycemic index. Intellectually, the glycemic 

index was an important idea. It followed the same principle as 

low-carbohydrate diets, and was seemingly of practical value. 

The intention of the glycemic index was to address the experi¬ 

mental effect of carbohydrate on blood glucose. The glycemic 

index addresses the old idea, pretty much a dogma when I was 

in school, that simple sugars would cause a rapid rise in blood 

glucose, but complex carbohydrate—which at that time still 

meant polysaccharides (starch)—would not. The idea was ques¬ 

tioned at some point, however, and it turns out that when you 

actually measure the effect of foods on blood glucose, it’s not 

easily predictable—that is, it must be determined experimen¬ 

tally. Glycemic index (GI) is precisely defined as the area under 

the blood glucose time curve during the first two hours after 

consumption of 50 grams of carbohydrate-containing food. In 

other words, it is the total amount of blood glucose for a fixed 

time period after ingestion. 


Whatever its promise, low-GI diets have evolved to be a 

politically correct form of carbohydrate restriction, and it is 

questionable if they have any value at all. Eric Westman, who 

has experience with both kinds of diets, put it well: “If low-GI 

is good, why not no-GI?” 


GI is mainly influenced by the absolute concentration of 

glucose in the food, the extent to which glucose appears in the 

blood (not necessarily from the food itself), and the quantity 

of other nutrients such as fat or fiber in the carbohydrate- 

containing food that might slow the digestion or absorption 

of carbohydrate. The individual personal variation makes it 



Whaddaya Know? 



47 



doubtful that GI diets are useful at all. In comparison to simply 

reducing carbohydrate, low-GI strategies are complicated and 

require looking up and calculating values, a feature that might 

be appealing to some, but is probably annoying to most. 


The difference between intensive variables, such as caloric 

density, and extensive variables, such as total carbohydrate 

eaten, was brought out at the beginning of the quiz. GI is an 

intensive variable. Two bowls of cereal have the same GI as one. 

If there is not much carbohydrate (or really much glucose) in a 

food, it will have a low GI, but it could still have a large effect 

if you consume a lot. The glycemic load attempts to correct this 

problem. The glyccmic load (GL) is defined as the GI multiplied 

by the grams of carbohydrate in a sample of a particular food. 

Obviously, GL is still an intensive variable. You still have to 

know how much is consumed. More important, are you really 

sure that GL will really be different from total carbohydrate, 

which is easier to calculate? 


There is also the overall character of using GL A slice of 

white bread has a high GI. The GI will go down if you smear a 

tablespoon of butter on the bread. It will go down still further 

if you add two tablespoons of butter. If you could somehow 

butter infinitely, until for all intents and purposes you have pure 

butter, you would have a GI = 0, which is probably not helpful 

for those who want to use the GI as a guide to eating. 


One final ambiguity: GI measures blood glucose. Fructose, a 

sugar of great current interest (because it is 50 percent of sucrose 

and slighdy more than 50 percent of high-fructose corn syrup), 

is partially converted to glucose in two hours, which is why the 

GI of fructose is 20 and not zero. In fact, more is converted after 

that time, severely compromising any assertion about the diff er¬ 

ences in effect of the two sugars. Sucrose has a GI of 70, wliich 

is roughly the average of glucose and fructose. Thus, ice cream 

has a lower GI than potatoes. Yet now we cant recommend ice 



48 



Nutrition in Crisis 



cream because of the high fructose. Lower GI or lower fructose? 

How can you do both without saying “low-carbohydrate” out 

loud? Tliis tangled web is woven out of the failure to face scien¬ 

tific facts. This aspect of the nutritional crisis is probably best 

addressed by ignoring glvcemic index altogether. 



Health, and National Institutes of Health (NIH)—has been on an antifat 

crusade for fifty years. No experiment will make them change their point 

of view—or more precisely, clearly describe their point of view, which they 

can apparently mold and reform to fit any challenge. Low-fat products are 

still everywhere and low-fat is still recommended in one way or another. 

The diet-heart hypothesis holds that lipid in your diet, particularly satu¬ 

rated fat, will raise one or another lipid fraction in your blood, which will 

in turn cause you to have a heart attack. TheyVe put it to the test—big, 

expensive clinical trials with tens of thousands of subjects, hundreds of 

millions of dollars—and it consistently fails. To figure out whats going on, 

and to avoid the path to poor health, you have to understand the details. 

Some of the details involve more chemistry. Many terms in the popular 

media are used incorrectly and contribute to the current crisis. A little 

precision will help you understand the problem. 


First, you need to know that the term lipid refers to a diverse collection 

of chemical compounds, all of which are sparingly or not at all soluble in 

water. The group includes fatty acids, fats and oils, cholesterol, and deriva¬ 

tives of these compounds. Directly applicable here are the fats and oils and 

their constituent components, the fatty acids and glycerol. WeTl look at fat 

structure and the meaning of saturated and unsaturated fat. 


The crux of the crisis is the packaging of lipids into complex units, the 

lipoproteins (LDL, HDL, and others) that transport cholesterol and fat. 

These particles are targeted as surrogate markers for cardiovascular risk. 

As surrogates, they are of limited value. The extent to which they truly 

indicate risk is controversial. Much of the revolution in nutrition revolves 

around facing the fact that a surrogate is not the same as actually getting 



Whaddaya Know? 



49 



sick. It is still important to be familiar with them, however. They will be 

part of your lipid workup when you get a clinical lab test. 


The big payoff will be wrapping your head around how fat interacts with 

carbohydrate, and looking ahead, we will try to understand how carbohy- 



drate can be converted to fat, but, to 


a large extent, fat cannot be converted 


to glucose. We will want to grasp how it is that we cannot use our fat stores 

to keep glucose at normal levels and how it is that the amount of dietary 

carbohydrate might be more important than the amount of dietary fat in 


determining how much body fat we 


have. Now, back to the quiz. 


7. A good source of monosaturated fat is: 


(check all that apply) 


Student response (%) 


□ butter 



M canola oil 


22 


□ corn oil 


25 


□ flaxseed oil 


26 


0 olive oil 


58 


IE) avocado oil 


38 


□ soybean oil 


18 



You hear the terms saturated fat and polyunsaturated fat often, but they 

are not quite precise; only fatty acids can be unsaturated or saturated. All 

dietary and body fats and oils are triglycerides (TG), or, more correcdy tria- 

cylglycerols (TAG).The name tells you about the structure: There are three 

acyl groups (pronounced “ay-seal”). Acyl is the adjective form of acid, and 

the components are fatty acids whose three acyl groups are attached to the 

compound glycerol. Fats have an is-shaped structure. The three arms of the 

E are the fatty acids, and the backbone is the compound glycerol. As shown 

in figure 2.1, only the fatty acids can be saturated (SFAs) or unsaturated 

(UFAs). Saturatedfat simply means that the fat contains a higher propor¬ 

tion of saturated fatty acids. For unsaturated fat and its variations, mono- or 

poly-, the chemical bonds that attach the fatty acids to the glycerol are 

called ester bonds. You only need to know the term ester because when the 

fatty acids are found alone, especially in blood, they are referred to as free 

fatty acids (FFAs) or—because they are no longer attached to the glycerol 

part by the ester bonds—nonesterified fatty acids (NEFAs). So FFA and 



50 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Glycerol 




Fatty acid 


Fatty acyl- (adjective) 



Q Carbon 

Oxygen 

Hydrogen 










Triglyceride (TAG) 


Figure 2.2. Fat structure. Fats and oils are triglycerides (TGs), formally triacylglycerol 

(TAGs). There are three ester bonds to glycerol. 



Stearic Acid-Saturated Fatty Acid 




Oleic Acid-Monounsaturated Fatty Acid 




Linoleic Acid-Polyunsaturated Fatty Acid 




Figure 2.3. Single and double chemical bonds as seen in common fatty acids. 





Whaddaya Know? 



51 



Trans isomer 




“across” 



★ 


A=A trans-2-butene 



Cis isomer 



“adjacent” 




cis-2-butene 



Figure 2.4. Orientation around double chemical bonds. The two carbons in a double 

bond each have a hydrogen atom and another atom, a carbon, or in the case of fatty 

acid, a carbon chain. If these are on the same side of the double bond, this constitutes 

the cis- configuration. Otherwise, the bonds are called trans-. 



NEFA are the same thing. Fatty acids are long chains of carbon atoms 

with a carboxylic acid group. The fatty acids provide the real fuel in fat in 

the long hydrocarbon chains, much like gasoline. Carbon—carbon double 

bonds are more chemically reactive and can be converted to single bonds 

(e.g., with hydrogen atoms, in which case they are called saturated—that 

is, saturated with hydrogen). Saturated means that all the carbon-carbon 

chemical bonds are single bonds. 


Saturated fats, again, have a high percentage of SFAs in the arms of 

the E structure. Similarly, unsaturated fats have high amounts of MUFAs 

(monounsaturated fatty acids) and PUFAs (polyunsaturated fatty acids). 

For some fats, however, it is not clear that these terms are useful. One 

thinks of lard as a kind of pure high-saturated fat, but in fact it is only 41 

percent saturated and mostly (47 percent) MUFA, predominantly oleic 

acid, the main fat in olive oil. From the survey, our students did not have 

a good sense of what MUFA was. So it is a question of whether you think 

that lard is half full of SFA or half empty. Most important, as in the epide¬ 

miological study described in the introduction, it has been impossible to 

demonstrate any risk for cardiovascular disease associated with consuming 

saturated fat—and yet official health agencies continue to insist that there 

is. If anything, the studies show that carbohydrate is a greater risk than 

saturated fat, but both correlations are too weak to attribute a significant 

role to either. I will explore this more as we go along. 



52 



Nutrition in Crisis 



The structure of the different kinds of fatty acids are shown in figure 

2.3. The major monounsaturated fatty acid is oleic acid. Everybody thinks 

that monounsaturated fats—those with a high content of MUFA, such as 

olive oil—are protective of cardiovascular disease, but it is not so clear-cut. 


Few fats have only SFAs. Coconut oil is the exception, but those are 

medium-chain fatty acids (12—16 carbons)—that is, the arms of the E in 

figure 2.3 are shorter than the more common fatty acids. Because of the 

interest in ketogenic diets, coconut oil has recently become more popular: 

Medium-chain fatty acids form ketone bodies more readily. In reaction, 

and presumably because they don’t want the public drawing their own 

conclusions, the American Heart Association has decreed that coconut 

oil is dangerous, without providing any significant proof of this claim. 

Consumer Reports and the popular media have echoed the AHAs opinions, 

as if the AHA had come upon a new experimental discovery. 


Saturated fats (again, those with a fairly large number of SFAs) tend 

to be solid, while those with more UFAs tend to be liquid—generally, the 

more saturation the higher the melting point. To understand why, we need 

to look deeper into the structure of fatty acids, which brings us to the issue 

of trans-fats. 


Trans-Fats and the Meaning of Trans- 


When vegetable oils are hydrogenated—the process by which some of the 

unsaturated fatty acids are turned to saturated—a side reaction can occur 

that changes the configuration of some of the unsaturated fatty acids from 

cis- to trans-. 


Let me first explain what trans- means, since the nutritional Murphy’s 

Law dictates that confusion will be introduced wherever possible. The 

carbon-carbon double bond has rigid geometry, so that unlike the single 

bond, there is no rotation around the bond. Imagine a chain of carbon 

atoms as in a hydrocarbon such as gasoline or the backbone of a fatty acid. 

If the bonds are all saturated (single) bonds, then you can think of the 

molecule as somewhat floppy because of free rotation around the bonds. If 

there is a double bond in this chain, however, there are two ways to arrange 

the structure: The two carbons in a double bond can have hydrogen atoms 

on the same side of the bond (cis-) or on opposite sides (trans-). Figure 2.4 

shows the geometry around double bonds. 



Whaddaya Know? 



53 



Almost all naturally occurring fatty acids have the cis- configuration, 

but it is important to understand that, by itself, this is just a designation 

for the millions of double bond-containing compounds in the world. 

Figure 2.3 shows that SFAs have less structure than UFAs, and this lack 

of structure is why saturated fats tend to be solid: They are easier to pack 

into a solid. (By analogy, it is easier to pack T-shirts into a box than to pack 

model Eiffel Towers or heads of Nefertiti.) 



MUFAs and the Mediterranean Diet 


The Mediterranean diet is widely recommended for its health 

benefits, but the data are very weak—if there are any at all—and it 

is not obvious that anybody knows what the diet consists of beyond 

pouring olive oil on everything. The idea probably originated with 

Ancel Keys, generally considered the father of lipophobia. Keys 

originally found a good correlation between tat consumption 

(actually fat availability) in six different countries and the incidence 

of heart disease in diose countries. 5 It wasn’t long, however, before 

the Secretary of Health in New York State and a professor at 

Berkeley published a paper showing that there were data from 

countries other than the six that Keys had studied, which would 

have significantly weakened the correlation. 6 Keys has generally 

been characterized as a zealot, although he was probably more 

open-minded than some of his followers. He was, however, not 

easily embarrassed and undertook a second study of seven countries. 


The Seven Countries Study on dietary availability of fat had 

an interesting result: The two countries with the highest intake 

of fat were Finland, which had the highest incidence of CVD, 

and Crete, which had the lowest 7 It was deduced that this had 

to do with the type of fat: saturated in the case of Finland, 

and unsaturated, in the case of Crete. Tilings were further 

complicated when it was later pointed out that there were large 

differences m CVD between different areas of Finland that had 



54 



Nutrition in Crisis 



the same diet. This information was ignored by Keys, a pioneer 

in such approaches to dealing with conflicting data. In any case, 

the finding immediately led to the recommendations to lower 

saturated fat, although for most people there was a lingering 

idea that it was good to reduce fat across the board, despite the 

lack of correlation in the Seven Countries Study. Subsequently, 

health agencies were stronger in stepping up the pressure on 

saturated fat, but not so good at admitting the error in their 

earlier recommending of low-fat across the board. This is still 

the state of affairs. The real problem, however, is that even the 

link between saturated fat and heart disease has been impossible 

to establish: Direct tests failed immediately and continue to fail. 

The story of the political triumph of an idea that was clearly 

contradicted by the science has been told numerous times, 8 and 

yet the phenomenon still persists. Every time you see a low-fat 

item in the supermarket, you are looking at an artifact of one of 

the most bizarre stories in the history of science. 


One of the rarely cited responses to die Seven Countries 

Study was a letter written by researchers at the University of 

Crete and published in the journal Public Health Nutrition: The 

important part of the letter is this: 


In the December 2004 issue of your journal . . . 

Geoffrey Cannon referred to ... the fact that Keys and 

his colleagues seemed to have ignored the possibility 

that Greek Orthodox Christian fasting practices could 

have influenced the dietary habits of male Cretans in 

the 1960s. ... Professor Aravanis confirmed that, in 

die 1960s, 60% of the study participants were fasting 

during the 40 days of Lent , and stricdy followed all 

fasting periods of the church . .. periodic abstention 

from meat , fishy dairy products , eggs and cheese^ as well 

as abstention from olive oil consumption on certain 





Whaddaya Know? 



55 



Wednesdays and Fridays ... this was not noted in the 

study, and no attempt was made to differentiate between 

fosters and non-fasters. In our view this was a remark¬ 

able and troublesome omission, (Emphasis added) 


The whole sorry tale has now been told many times, most 

recently and completely by Nina Teicholz in her expose of the 

low-fat fiasco, The Big Fat Surprise} 1 



Returning to the Composition of Dietary Fat 


Going back to figure 2.3, the composition of different dietary fats turns 

out to be somewhat surprising. It is true that there is a lot of oleic acid 

(the major monounsaturated fatty acid) in olive oil (73 percent) and canola 

oil (58 percent). Less well known is that the highest amount is found in 

avocado oil—and probably most surprising is that oleic acid makes up 

almost half of the fatty acids in beef tallow and lard (44 percent and 47 

percent, respectively). Beef tallow (rendered fat) was what McDonalds 

used to use to fry their French fries in—at the time, they got the thumbs 

up from Julia Child—until they were pressured to switch to vegetable oil 

in a movement spearheaded by Michael Jacobson. Currently the executive 

director of the Center for Science in the Public Interest, Michael Jacobson 

might best be described as humorless, uptight, and puritanical. I have been 

accused of inappropriate behavior in making this characterization, but 

you can check out his interview with Stephen Colbert for proof (Colbert: 

“What is the latest thing that you’re warning people not to enjoy?”) Of 

course, when McDonald s did switch to vegetable oil, the amount of trans¬ 

fat went up, and that got Jacobson riled up again. In any case, most of the 

saturated fat in beef is stearic acid, which is considered neutral or “heart- 

healthy,” but the question is at least ambiguous—and again, beef fat is only 

half SFA; the other half is mostly oleic acid, as in the Mediterranean diet. 


Canola oil, it turns out, is named for CANadian Oil, Low Acid, an oil 

isolated from rapeseed {rape, from Latin for turnip in this context), which also 



56 



Nutrition in Crisis 



contains euricic acid. This fatty acid is the star of the 1992 film Lorenzo's Oil , 

but it is not as beneficial as the movie suggests. It is generally considered toxic, 

and for that reason, it is removed in making canola oil. My original vision of 

the Quebecoise in their native costumes picking canola fruit from the canola 

tree turned out not to be correct There is no canola tree, and the real source, 

the rapeseed plant, was originally processed to remove the euricic acid. 


The rapeseed plant has now been bred to have inherently low euricic 

acid, and there now really is a plant called canola, the oil of which is one of 

the major exports from Canada. Processing also produced trans-fatty acids, 

but this has been removed from current versions of the product. 


Back to the quiz. We asked students about the “cholesterol" species that 

are reported in your lipid profile and that are supposed to be indictors 

of risk of CVD. What is called cholesterol in this context is actually a 

supramolecular (more than one type of molecule) known as a lipoprotein, 

which contains lipid protein. 



Lipoproteins: “Good” and “Bad” Cholesterol 


There were a few questions from the original quiz that are not reprinted 

here. Most of our students knew that low-fat diets lower low-density 

lipoprotein cholesterol (LDL), the so-called “bad cholesterol.” LDL is 

considered bad because it is assumed to correlate well with heart disease, but 

the correlation is not as strong as many believe. Less than half the people 

who experience a first heart attack have high cholesterol, and less than half 

have high LDL (although high is, of course, subject to interpretation). 


Cholesterol is not a cause of heart disease—we don t know what the 

cause is—but total cholesterol is considered a very poor risk marker, 

although it is still treated as a factor associated with incidence of disease. 

There are, however, better risk markers, including triglycerides and the 

“good cholesterol,” HDL. 


8. The diet component that is most likely to 


raise triglycerides (fat in the blood) is: 


0 carbohydrate 


□ fat 


□ protein 



Student response (%) 



41 


19 



Whaddaya Know? 



57 



The phenomenon of carbohydrate-induced hypertriglyceridemia 

(high blood triglyceride) has been known for at least sixty years. A major 

contributor is the process of de novo fatty acid synthesis, more usually 

called de novo lipogenesis (DNL), in which fatty acids are made from 

other components, mainly carbohydrate. It is significant that the fatty 

acid that is made in DNL is palmitic acid, the sixteen-carbon saturated 

fatty acid. Bottom line: Carbohydrate in the diet raises saturated fat in 

the blood. This was demonstrated most convincingly by experiments at 

the University of Connecticut 11 described in chapter 7. These experiments 

revealed that saturated fatty acid in the blood might be harmful, but that 

its presence is more dependent on the intake of dietary carbohydrate than 

dietary fat. Our students completely missed the boat on this one. 


How much of a risk factor is high triglycerides? Well, it is impossible 

to tell. The AHA tends to downplay the importance of triglycerides. This 

is probably related to the need to avoid talking about low-carbohydrate 

diets, since dietary carbohydrate restriction is the most effective method 

of reducing high triglycerides except perhaps for total starvation. The 

AHA has been pretty clear on not endorsing low-carbohydrate diets. On 

the other hand, triglycerides have clearly become a focus of the ADA, 

evidenced by their alarmist attitude toward sugar, or fructose in particular. 

Sugar is carbohydrate, and regardless of whether an increase in fructose is 

more or less effective than glucose in elevating triglycerides, both will raise 

triglycerides. 


9. In general, what effect does a low-fat diet 

have on HDL-C (high density lipoprotein 


cholesterol, the “good cholesterol”)? Student response (%) 


□ increase 31 


EE] decrease 39 


□ no change 30 



A low-fat diet reduces cholesterol, both “good” and “bad.” The bottom 

line on the cholesterol problem: The literature tends to show that a 

subtype of the LDL particle, the smaller LDL, is generally found to be 

most atherogenic (contributing to a highest risk for CVD). High levels of 



58 



Nutrition in Crisis 



small, dense LDL are referred to as “pattern B” and this pattern is most 

dependent on the level of carbohydrate, rather than the level of fat. This 

critical observation has had little effect on official positions of the AHA 

or other agencies. It would appear that they don’t think LDL size matters, 

but they have not said exactly why not. The AHA did, however, in 2000, 

quietly remove their proscription against total fat. You didnt know that? 


LDL particle size is not generally measured in a standard lipid profile, 

and your physician is most likely to look at total cholesterol, or total LDL 

cholesterol, to determine if you are at risk for heart disease. The recognized 

surrogate for pattern B is the ratio of triglycerides to HDL. The cutoff is 

3.5. 12 If your value is below that mark, you are at limited risk for cardio¬ 

vascular disease. 


It is assumed that the reduction in triglycerides and increase in HDL 

that is a consequence of a low-carbohydrate diet has a protective effect. As 

suggested in the introduction, however, it is possible that, outside of well- 

defined genetic abnormalities, what you eat might have no effect on your 

risk of heart disease—or, more precisely, individual variations that might 

predict a link between diet and heart disease have not yet been discov¬ 

ered. We will come back to this theme. The response to doubt separates 

science from medicine and religion. It is revolutionary to even consider 

the possibility that the diet-heart hypothesis as it s currently presented is 

completely wrong. In science, doubt represents an opportunity. In religion, 

you pray for relief from doubt. 



10. The dietary change that is most likely to 


increase the risk of cardiovascular disease is: Student response (%) 



□ unsaturated fat saturated fat 

0 unsaturated fat -> carbohydrate 


□ carbohydrate -> unsaturated fat 


□ carbohydrate saturated fat 


□ saturated fat -> carbohydrate 


□ saturated fat unsaturated fat 



25 


28 


12 


19 


16 




This is one of the most important observations, because it has been 

known for so long. I described in the introduction my early research into 

the literature and my dismay at the results of the Nurses’ Health Study, 13 



Whaddaya Know? 



59 



which demonstrated that there was an increase in CVD incidence when 

fat was replaced by carbohydrate, regardless of the type of fat being 

replaced. This was astounding given the persistent low-fat message. One 

would expect that the study, done more than fifteen years ago, would 

have been the stimulus for a shift away from that low-fat message. That 

didn't happen. 



Assessing the Quiz Results 


How did you do? Some critical facts were not known to most of our medi¬ 

cal students. In summary, whatever you knew before, the information that 

you should take forward is this: 


• The majority of the increase in calories in the epidemic of obesity and 

diabetes has been due to a dramatic increase in carbohydrate consump¬ 

tion. The association between the observed behavior of the population 

in macronutrient consumption and official advice to reduce fat and 

increase carbohydrates might be causal. How could the advice not have 

played a role? The bottom line, that reducing carbohydrate is the best 

treatment for diabetes, suggests that carbohydrate must have played 

some role in the origins of the epidemic, but this remains unknown. 

What is established is that the progression of culprits—saturated fat, 

red meat, white rice—that are “proved” daily by epidemiologic studies 

to be causes can probably be excluded. 


• The crux of the problem in controlling the epidemic of diabetes can be 

summarized in the following statements: 


• Dietary carbohydrates raise blood glucose in people with diabetes 

more than other macronutrients. 


• There is no biological requirement for carbohydrate (for anybody). 


• Despite evidence to the contrary, health agencies recommend high 

carbohydrates (more than 40 percent of total calories). 


• The glycemic index and glycemic load are a weak form of low-carb strategy 

The logical problems and the limited experimental proof of their efficacy 

make their use questionable as a primary strategy. They might, however, 

be of some use, since they still encourage carbohydrate restriction. 



60 



Nutrition in Crisis 



• Calories are about processes, not substance, and looking ahead, different 

processes (oxidation in the calorimeter versus metabolism) make differ¬ 

ent use of the calories. 


• On the technical side: It’s good to pay attention to the difference between 

intensive properties, such as calorie density, and extensive properties, 

such as total calories. When you hear people say fat is inherently more 

fattening, you need to know that doesn’t mean anything. 


The major points about lipids from the second part of the quiz include: 


• The terms saturated and unsaturated can only be applied to fatty acids, 

the constituents of fats and oils. The composition of common fats and 

oils is different from popular conceptions (e.g., beef tallow is almost half 

oleic acid, the main fatty acid of olive oil.) 


• The dietary change that has the greatest effect on cardiovascular risk 

factors is replacement of fat with carbohydrate. Low-fat diets reduce 

LDL, but low-carbohydrate diets reduce the important subfractions, 

the pattern B, that are more atherogenic. Reducing carbohydrate also 

improves HDL. The real question, however, is whether reducing carbo¬ 

hydrate diminishes the actual incidence of CVD. It would be difficult 

to answer that question, but it must be considered. What you might not 

have known before the quiz is that our current state of knowledge does 

not provide evidence that what you eat will make any difference in your 

risk of heart disease. 


• The ambiguity or, more precisely, the near absolute failure of the diet- 

heart hypothesis contained the seeds of the first low-carb revolution. 

We’ll look at this in the next chapter, and I will explain why another 

revolution is needed. 



-CHAPTER 3- 



The First Low- 

Carbohydrate Revolution 


T he first low-carbohydrate revolution dates from about 2002. As is 

frequently the case in politics, the revolutionaries saw themselves 

as a loyal opposition and probably didn’t think of their ideas as 

particularly iconoclastic. Dr. Atkins was an established physician who was 

undoubtedly only trying to help. He was surprised at the vehement backlash. 

Unfortunately his response to criticism was less like that of John Adams 

than that of John’s cousin, Samuel Adams, who was described in Don’t 

Know Much About History as being better at brewing dissent than beer. It is 

doubtful that much could have been done, though. Like political revolutions, 

scientific revolutions usually have to be won more than once. Gary Wills 

described the Gettysburg Address as a statement that the Civil War was a 

second American Revolution. 1 People of my generation might see the civil 

rights movement as a third. It’s often the same for revolutions in science: 

We are taught that atomic theory comes from John Dalton, the Manchester 

schoolteacher who proposed it in 1799, but atoms were not truly accepted as 

real things, rather than convenient models, until Einstein nailed it in 1905. 


The idea that dietary carbohydrate, sugars, and starches have some unique 

power to make animals fat is very old. It would be hard to identify the first 

farmer who fattened animals for market by feeding them grain. Brillat-Savarin, 

the father of modern gourmet cooking, generalized the fattening principle to 

human beings, and claimed that there were folks who were “carbophores”—in 

fact, he admitted to being one himself. 2 The mechanism—the anabolic effects 

of the hormone insulin, stimulated primarily by the sugar glucose—was a 

well-established physiologic phenomenon before the first low-carbohydrate 

revolution. The scientific literature provides many examples of weight loss 

from carbohydrate restriction and links it to something beyond the reduction 




62 



Nutrition in Crisis 



in calories that usually comes with such a diet. Although it has been around 

in one form or another for a long time, carbohydrate restriction only became 

revolutionary with the ascendancy of a kind of low-fat nutritional-medical 

monarchy, backed by powerful influences. 


The first edition of the original Atkins book 3 appeared in the 1970s, 

right around the time of the codification of low-fat as the desirable diet. 

The Atkins diet was so vehemently denounced that it gave rise to congres¬ 

sional hearings. An amusing moment occurred when the American 

Medical Association (AMA) asserted that one of the dangers of a low- 

carbohydrate diet for weight loss was that it might lead to anorexia. Overall 

it was an indication of the unwillingness of the medical professions to 

tolerate dissent. 


As in political revolutions, the first low-carbohydrate revolution was 

stimulated by a kind of manifesto, a document that historians now describe 

as being a call to action. The equivalent of Thomas Paine s Common Sense , 

which fired everybody up for the American Revolution, was a 2002 article 

by Gary Taubes in the New York Times Magazine titled “What If It s All 

Been a Big Fat Lie.” 4 Later expanded into the book Good Calories, Bad 

Calories , s the article documented the political ascendancy of the low-fat 

paradigm and the establishment of something like the Court of Low~Fat. 


The AMA, the AHA, and a number of influential physicians were all 

received at court. The media and the government, including the McGovern 

committee, went along with it. Tom Naughtons comedy documentary Fat 

Heacf* includes a clip of McGovern explaining in 1977 that Congress did 

not have the luxury of waiting for all the science to be in. 


The McGovern hearings began a pattern of ignoring dissenting voices, 

like that of Philip Handler, head of the National Academy of Sciences, 

who testified that there was little science behind this rush to judgment. I 

recognized Handler as part of White, Handler, and Smith, the group of 

authors that wrote one of the few comprehensive biochemistry texts at the 

time—that is to say, he was a very well-known biochemist. 



The Lipophobes and Their Opposition 


There were many experimental and clinical trials that set out to prove 

the diet-heart hypothesis. The first, the Framingham Study, which 



The First Low-Carbohydrate Revolution 



63 



continues today, is a massive survey of the behavior of residents in this large 

Massachusetts town. The original results showed no effect of dietary total 

fat, saturated fat, or cholesterol on CVD. The study did initially find an 

association between blood cholesterol and CVD, but the correlation wasn't 

a knockout, and it became weaker as the study continued. This original posi¬ 

tive but unsustainable association is now a classic in epidemiology, taught in 

statistics classes. The fact that diet did not correlate with CVD is less often 

discussed, and in the later data on cholesterol, some age groups (men, 48-57 

years old) actually displayed greater risk with lower cholesterol. 


The results of the Framingham Study were buried for years until the 

statistician Tavia Gordon had them published in 1968, almost ten years 

after the data had been analyzed. Publication of the data should have been 

the death of diet-heart hypothesis right there. If fat was as bad as they 

said, there wouldn't have been a single study that failed to prove it. Not 

one. In actuality, almost every one of the dozen or so large trials conducted 

since Framingham has also failed. Science, however, was not the major 

force behind the denial of evidence. The lipophobes, as Michael Pollan 

calls them, 7 continued to dismiss each experimental failure as the loss of 

a minor battle in a war where victory would surely fall to them: just one 

more big clinical trial, just another hundred million bucks and you will see 

how bad fat is. Even in 2001, when the AHA removed its proscriptions 

against total dietary fat, it was done without fanfare. As a result, it is likely 

that most consumers think that AHA still recommends reduction in over¬ 

all dietary fat. They're still down on saturated fat, and of course, trans-fat 

(the latter being a moot issue since it's been almost entirely removed from 

the food supply), but the truth is that the AHA has given up on total fat. 


Although preceded by other exposes, Taubes's Good Calories, Bad 

Calories was the most compelling presentation of how nutritional science 

had been taken over by lipophobes. Numerous retellings have followed. 

Nina Teicholz's The Big Fat Surprise* is of comparable literary quality to 

Good Calories, Bad Calories , and is more explicit in its condemnations of 

the players. Ultimately, with control over the NIH, the low-fat mafia could 

now resist all scientific argument and dismiss all of the experimental fail¬ 

ures to provide any sort of reasonable case. The ascendancy of low-fat was, 

and still is, coupled with a special hatred for low-carbohydrate diets and 

especially for their main exponent, Dr. Atkins, even after his death. 



64 



Nutrition in Crisis 



The low-fat idea wasn’t good to begin with, but of all the tests of the 

idea that failed, one after another, nothing was more embarrassing than 

the Womens Health Initiative (WHI), which reported in 2006: “Over 

a mean of 8.1 years, a dietary intervention that reduced total fat intake 

and increased intakes of vegetables, fruits, and grains did not significantly 

reduce the risk of coronary heart disease (CHD), stroke, or CVD in post¬ 

menopausal women.” A multicenter, nearly $400 million study, the WHI 

had assigned 19,541 postmenopausal women to the dietary intervention 

and had a control group of29,294 women in a free-living setting. As such, 

its failure should have been a bombshell. 


It was not long before Dr. Elizabeth Nabel, director of the National 

Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute of the NIH, appeared on television to 

assure the nation that the recommendations had not changed regardless 

of the study’s findings. You really did still need to reduce saturated fat, 

she insisted. Nothing’s changed, despite the study they funded explicitly 

showing that change was needed. 


The situation was serious. The refusal to accept the failure of a scientific 

test, and the stubborn insistence on doctrine, caused palpable harm. The 

WHI women weren’t getting any better, and the population at large, doing 

its best to adhere to low-fat, was getting fatter and more diabetic during 

this period. Refusal to see the WHI for what it was represented a clear 

statement that the lipophobe movement, starting at the top at the NIH, 

was going to stonewall any effort to change. 


“The Shot Heard ’Round the World” 


IfTaubes’s “What If It’s All Been a Big Fat Lie” was the Common Sense 

of the first low-carbohydrate revolution, then the “shot heard ’round the 

world” was the report by Gary Foster and coworkers 9 showing that the 

Atkins diet actually improved markers for cardiovascular disease, the lipo- 

phobes’ main “concern” about low-carbohydrate diets. 


Foster’s demonstration had a big impact because he spoke for the whole 

nutritional establishment. He later described, in public lectures, how 

he and his collaborators had been having lunch at a scientific meeting, 

bemoaning their inability to sweep the Atkins diet from their sight. They 

decided to get a grant to trash the diet, and so they did. One suspects that 



The First Low-Carbohydrate Revolution 



65 



their intent was clear in the grant application: not to test the efficacy of 

the Atkins diet—that wouldVe been nearly impossible to fund—but to 

show just how bad it was. So they carried out a one-year study comparing 

a low-carbohydrate diet modeled on the Atkins diet with a low-fat diet. 

What they found, contrary to the conclusion they’d hope for, was that “the 

low-carbohydrate diet produced a greater weight loss (absolute difference, 

approximately 4 percent) than did the conventional diet for the first six 

months.”This part was not a surprise even to the authors. Everybody knew 

somebody who had lost a lot of weight on the Atkins diet, and nutritionists 

had more or less accepted the idea that low-carbohydrate diets were good 

for weight loss, though they usually insisted that it was “just a reduction 

in calories.” (If youVe tried to lose weight, you know that there is no “just” 

about it.) The kicker, however, was that Foster reported: 


After three months, no significant differences were found 

between the groups in total or low-density lipoprotein cholesterol 

concentrations. The increase in high-density lipoprotein cholesterol 

concentrations and the decrease in triglyceride concentrations were 

greater among subjects on the low-carbohydrate diet than among 

those on the conventional diet throughout most of the study. 


Both diets significantly decreased diastolic blood pressure and 

the insulin response to an oral glucose load. 10 (Emphasis added) 


In other words, the low-carbohydrate diet was better on HDL (“good 

cholesterol”) and especially on triglycerides. Most importantly, there was 

no increase in LDL, which is what your doctor uses as the traffic light for 

determining whether you need to be prescribed statins. 



More Work Needs to Be Done 


The conclusion of the Foster study: “Longer and larger studies are required 

to determine the long-term safety and efficacy of low-carbohydrate, high- 

protein, high-fat diets.” This strange conclusion indicates the persistent 

difficulty in making progress. Low-fat diets do worse on most markers, 

and are at best a draw on the others, yet we are supposed to be worried 

about the low-carbohydrate diet? If the low-fat diet is worse, shouldn’t 



66 



Nutrition in Crisis 



we be worried about long-term safety and efficacy of that diet instead? 

Somehow the conclusion consistently drawn from Foster s experiment was 

that the “diets were the same at one year”—that it was a draw. There are 

probably sporting events where the champion keeps the title in the case 

of a draw, but the idea that the low-fat diet was some kind of champion 

with a long-term record of success is absurd. The allegedly “prudent” and 

“moderate” low-fat diet is exactly the one that gave us the epidemic of 

obesity and diabetes in the first place. 



Ad Lib Versus Calorie Restriction 


Beyond the obvious bias, Foster's study was compromised in its experi¬ 

mental design. People in the low-fat group were directed to consume an 

explicitly low-calorie diet: They were required to eat what they were told. In 

the low-carbohydrate group, on the other hand, participants were allowed 

to eat anything that they wanted as long as they kept carbohydrates low. 

(Even if you believe that the diets were actually equal, which diet would 

you go for?) This protocol was used because Foster et al. were not testing 

the principle of reducing insulin fluctuations as a means of controlling 

metabolism. The study was not testing, as in the title, “a low-carbohydrate 

diet for obesity.” Instead it was testing the Atkins diet and perhaps, in the 

authors' minds, Atkins himself. (After all, the Atkins diet said that you 

didn't have to count calories—anathema to traditional nutritionists.) The 

experiment, in reality, was testing two principles: that carbohydrate restric¬ 

tion means greater satiety, allowing you to regulate calories implicitly; and 

that the Atkins diet, calorie for calorie, is more effective for weight loss. 

Testing both at once was more demanding than isolating the variables. 

The low-carbohydrate diet's better results might have been due to either or 

both principles, but its impossible to know because of the flawed experi¬ 

mental design. 


The dietary protocol was not the only problem. Data were analyzed 

according to a bizarre method known as intention-to-treat (ITT). In this 

method, data from the subjects who had dropped out of the study were 

included in the results by “imputing” values based on previous measure¬ 

ments. The difference between “imputing” and making stuff up is hard to 

figure out. ITT doesn't make any more sense than you'd think from this 



The First Low-Carbohydrate Revolution 



67 



brief description, but that’s simply how things are (see chapter 16 for a 

more in-depth discussion). Beyond the obvious lack of common sense, an 

ITT will always make the better diet look worse than it actually is. In this 

event, though, those in favor of carbohydrate restriction were sufficiently 

happy to see the positive outcome, and they were disinclined to be too 

critical of the methods. At face value, the lipophobes had their shot and 

they lost. They tried to maintain a facade of impartiality while still putting 

the burden of proof on low-carbohydrate, but it still didn’t help their case. 

So, what happened to the first low-carbohydrate revolution? Why didn’t it 

move forward? How did low-fat loyalists prevail in the face of such strong 

scientific evidence? 



What Stopped the Revolution? 


It was an opportunity. The public had a chance to see if the low- 

carbohydrate idea would work. Many did try it, and many had 

great success. Popular articles were written about the phenomenon. 

Low-carbohydrate wasn’t equally effective for everyone, but frequently, 

it seemed miraculous. People described how the “pounds melted away.” 

So why didn’t it move forward? First, there was poor understanding of 

what actually made the diet work; and second, there was a proliferation 

of products designed to make low-carbohydrate “easier,” because it was 

perceived, incorrectly for the most part, as a difficult diet strategy. This 

allowed the company Atkins Nutritionals and other similar ventures to 

sell a lot of products, many of which were not always helpful. They are 

still doing that. These products help some dieters stay on track, but their 

artificial character made them suspicious, and the move toward natural 

food has made their reputation worse. The low-carbohydrate community 

itself mostly emphasized low-carbohydrate and, oddly, there was little 

disappointment when Atkins Nutritionals declared bankruptcy in 2005, 

though it has since been resurrected and continues to offer substitutes for 

the carbohydrates that you are giving up. Particularly troubling was the 

proliferation of products containing sugar alcohols—carbohydrates that 

are digested slowly, if at all, and were therefore presumed to not contrib¬ 

ute to blood glucose. Untested and poorly understood, sugar alcohols 

gave some people intestinal problems, but more importantly, they cast 



68 



Nutrition in Crisis 



what should have been a straightforward diet in a slightly bizarre light. 

Yet in the end, it was not Atkins Nutritionals or any other company, but 

the nutritionists and the professors of medicine who stopped the first 

low-carbohydrate revolution. They had a million objections and they got 

the media on their side. 


The Problem with Dietitians 


What was remarkable about the whole state of affairs was that the low-fat 

strategy had failed in competition with a real alternative. Low-fat could 

not compete with low-carbohydrate, even with the experiment set up in 

its favor and the authors putting a positive spin on the data. It was a direct 

challenge to nutritional orthodoxy, but the stalwarts of the old nutritional 

order did not go gentle into that good night. What torpedoed the first 

low-carbohydrate revolution were the nutritionists. They had the chance to 

tell the public, Tf you do want to try a low-carbohydrate diet, this is what 

we recommend.” Instead, they acted as if Fosters paper had never existed 

and they ignored those studies further supporting carbohydrate restriction 

that followed it. 


Nutrition has never been highly thought of. The field derives from the 

practical job of making institutional menus. The advances of physiology 

and biochemistry meant that nutrition increasingly overlapped with more 

solid science, but the field was, and is, very slow to change. Nutritionists 

have attempted to put on the mantle of professionalism. They are currently 

trying to establish the newly renamed Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics 

(AND), and to secure their status as the sole voice on nutrition, with the 

right to legally repress anybody else. Recently they tried to stop low- 

carbohydrate proponent Steve Cooksey from “offering counseling” on his 

blog despite appropriate disclaimers. Thankfully the courts decided that 

the First Amendment was still the law of the land and left Cooksey s 

accusers with egg white on their faces. However, the case, now replayed in 

several similarly ugly conflicts and described in later chapters, highlights 

the adversarial state of nutritional science and the intolerance of dissent. 

For those familiar with computer logic and with a taste for computer-nerd 

humor, my own group may rename itself OR (Objective Research) and 

plans to refer to the Cooksey affair as NAND-gate. 



The First Low-Carbohydrate Revolution 



69 



Underlying all of the resistance is the idea that only long-term, large- 

scale studies are important, an inaccurate assumption that nutritionists use 

as a basis for ignoring the smaller studies that are better controlled and 

provide more information. Small studies are not worse—they are generally 

better. More important, the quality of a study is not simply determined 

by the size or length of the study. But once again, the implicit assump¬ 

tion remains that the long-term studies have supported a low-fat diet—a 

prudent diet, a diet of moderation, a diet with proven success, a diet that 

can work for all of us. There is no such diet and there never was. The long¬ 

term studies have failed, almost every .single one: the Framingham Study, 

the Oslo Diet Heart Study, 11 the Western Electric Study, 12 The Women's 

Health Initiative, 13 and probably two dozen others. They showed no value 

in reducing dietary fat or saturated fat for prevention of heart disease or any 

other health conditions, and in the biggest trial of all, the “full-population 

trial” of Americans during the obesity epidemic, it was increased carbohy¬ 

drate, not fat, that was actually harmful. 


To be fair to nutritionists, doctors also played a part in the deception. 

Undeterred by their lack of training or experience in biochemistry or 

nutrition, it was de rigueur for junior faculty in a department of medicine 

to write a review trashing the Atkins diet. Some of these critiques even 

stated that the main flaw of low-carbohydrate diets was their failure to 

conform to the USDA dietary guidelines and other institutional recom¬ 

mendations. In other words, they faulted a diet whose central premise was 

that the USDA recommendations were bad, for not conforming to those 

recommendations. This is a known logical fallacy that was called “begging 

the question” before the phrase lost its original meaning: using the ques¬ 

tion as part of the answer. 


Failure to Accept Failure 


Stepping back and looking at the big picture, the most striking thing was 

the inability of low-fat diets, even those low-fat diets that did lower choles¬ 

terol, to provide a significant impact on cardiovascular outcome, or really, 

on anything else. Very large, very expensive clinical trials of low-fat dietary 

strategies failed, and yet our tax dollars continue to pay for similar trials. 

Even in those cases where we didn't have outcome data about heart attacks 



70 



Nutrition in Crisis 



or deaths, and instead had to look at the risk factors—the different choles¬ 

terol forms: HDL, LDL, and their subfractions—it turned out that the 

effect of reduced fat was at best ambiguous, whereas dietary carbohydrate 

typically had the major effect. As carbohydrates were increased, most of the 

risk markers got worse. (I should note that these markers and their associa¬ 

tion with outcome were not sufficient to attribute cause, but that had not 

stopped such interpretations when it was low-fat that reduced risk factors.) 


If we go beyond the original idea of total blood cholesterol as a major 

risk factor (less than half of the people who have a first heart attack have 

high cholesterol), and if we instead look at the different forms of the 

lipoprotein particles that are actually measured in clinical tests of blood 

cholesterol, carbohydrate restriction becomes the “default diet,” the one to 

try first for general health. It is important to acknowledge, however, that 

while low-carbohydrate diets look better for CVD risk factors, we have the 

same problem that we have with fat: There is little in the way of evidence 

that lowering carbohydrate can actually prevent CVD. Given its success 

in treatment of the collection of health markers referred to as metabolic 

syndrome, it would be surprising if reducing carbohydrate did not help in 

prevention—but at this point, what we know is very little. We are left with 

the real possibility that there is nothing at all to the diet—heart hypothesis, 

and that diet might not be a major player at all in CVD, except in cases of 

well-defined genetic conditions. Its very surprising, given our current view 

of things, and its likely to change as we learn more—but you have to go 

with the data. 



PART 2 


Nutrition and 

Metabolism 




-CHAPTER 4 — 



Basic Nutrition 


Macronutrients 



C arbohydrate, fat, and protein represent the major macronutrients 

because of the large quantities in which they are consumed. 

Micronutrients include vitamins and minerals, which are only 

taken in small amounts. It has become common to refer to foods as either 

“nutrient-rich” or “nutrient-dense,” or not, according to whether they re 

thought to have high amounts of micronutrients. Some people, including 

me, are annoyed by this lack of precision. After all, macronutrients are also 

nutrients, and it would make more sense to say “micronutrient-rich” if that 

is the intended meaning. Readers should be aware of this lack of precision 

moving forward. 


This chapter describes the primacy of macronutrient composition for 

metabolic effects. During the years from 1970 to 2000, roughly the period 

when observers began to notice an obesity epidemic, people consumed 

an excessive number of calories, the majority of which came from carbo¬ 

hydrates. The total amount of protein, usually the most stable part of the 

American diet, did not change. Fat, if anything, went down. 



The Basics of Carbohydrate Chemistry 


Chemically, the class of compounds called carbohydrates includes: simple 

sugars (monosaccharides), such as glucose and fructose; combinations of 

two simple sugars (disaccharides), such as sucrose, which is made up of one 

glucose and one fructose; polymers (polysaccharides), such as starch and 

glycogen; and derivatives of the sugars (e.g., the so-called sugar alcohols). 


Alcohol—that is, ethanol—is not a carbohydrate despite what you 

might hear on YouTube. Whatever the extent to which sugar can make 



74 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Starch {corn, wheat, potato, etc.) 



Sucrose (table sugar) 



Enzymatic 

digestion of 

corn starch 




Gl digestion 

starch 



## 




Gl digestion 



Glucose (e.g., corn syrup) 



Glucose 



Fructose 



Enzymatic 

conversion of Glucose, 


glucose to fructose Fructose Mixture 

(e.g., HFCS) 



Figure 4.1. Structure and transformations of the common carbohydrates. Starch is 

a polymer of glucose. In digestion, the glucose units are released and absorbed as 

such. Sucrose is a dimer of fructose and glucose, and digestion produces the two 

monosaccharides. 



you as loopy as alcohol, the two compounds are simply not the same 

on a chemical level. A horse is not a dog. One of the reasons that we 

make Pre-Meds study organic chemistry is in hope that the precision in 

naming organic compounds will carry over into pharmacology. It is likely 

that manufacturers started spelling klonopin (an antidepressant) with a 

k because they didn't want physicians to accidentally prescribe clonidine 

(an antihypertensive). 


In terms of formal chemistry, sugars are polyhydroxy aldehydes and 

ketones. The most common sugar is glucose, and it almost always cyclizes 

(folds up) in the form of a hexagon, at least in aqueous solutions. Fructose 

can also form a six-membered ring, but it is more likely to cyclize in a 

pentagonal shape, as shown in figure 4.1, which provides a simplified 

representation of the common sugars and related polymers. You can see 

from the figure that starch is a polymer of glucose (polysaccharide), and 

that it breaks down into simple sugars during digestion. If you never did 

the experiment in grade school, you can try chewing a piece of bread for 



Basic Nutrition 



75 



several minutes. The sweetness that develops is the result of digestive 

enzymes in saliva that catalyze the conversion of the bread’s starch into 

sugar (glucose). Not all starch molecules are as simple as the one depicted 

in figure 4.1: While some starch molecules, called amylose, do have a linear 

structure, other types, called amylopectin, have many branch points. 



Glycogen: Glucose Savings Account 


Glycogen is a highly branched polymer of glucose that serves as the stor¬ 

age and supply depot for body glucose flux. The liver, a kind of command 

center of metabolism, provides glucose for other organs, and muscle, the 

main consumer of glucose, stores glycogen for its own use. The liver has the 

highest concentration of glycogen, but there is more muscle tissue in the 

body, so it is muscle that possesses the highest total amount of glycogen. 


Glycogen is a dynamic storage site. The extensive branching means that 

there are a lot of ends from which glucose units can be chopped off as 

needed. It also means that glycogen occupies a lot of space. Children with 

one of the inborn errors of metabolism known as glycogen storage diseases 

will have visibly distended abdomens due to the liver increasing in size 

(hepatomegaly) to accommodate glycogen stores, which in these diseases 

can no longer be broken down. 


We think of glycogen as desirable because of its association with endur¬ 

ance in sporting events. This association is the basis for carbohydrate 

loading the day before a marathon, but, even in the area of athletics, things 

are not clear-cut: Marathons are mostly run on fat, even if you are not 

adapted to a low-carbohydrate diet. Jeff Volek and Steve Phinney have 

recently trained marathon runners and other elite athletes to perform on 

very low-carbohydrate ketogenic diets. The athletes do well. They win 

races, describe perception of greater stamina (not “hitting the wall”), and, 

surprisingly, they maintain glycogen stores—that is, they run on fat instead 

of depleting glycogen. It should be noted, however, that a ketogenic diet 

for athletes depends on a training and accommodation period. 


On a low-carbohydrate diet, glycogen storage tends to be reduced, typi¬ 

cally by around 60 percent with very low-carbohydrate intake (< 100 g/day). 

However, as I discussed earlier, metabolism does not run on mass action 

(how much is available) but rather on hormones and enzymes—so under 



76 



Nutrition in Crisis 



conditions of low-carbohydrate intake, glycogen will be replenished by the 

glucose produced from gluconeogenesis, thus maintaining storage. It is 

important to understand that gluconeogenesis and glycogen metabolism 

are really one process: Glucose synthesized from protein might be stored as 

glycogen and then only later appear in the blood. An important feature of 

a carbohydrate-restricted diet, however, is the switch to a metabolic state 

that runs on fat rather than carbohydrate. 


Glucose is at the center of metabolism. Looking ahead to chapter 5, 

the main theme in human biochemistry is that there are two major fuels: 

glucose and acetyl-CoA (derived largely from fat and pronounced ASS-a- 

teel Co-AY). Two fuels and two goals: provide energy and maintain blood 

glucose at a constant level. Too little blood glucose (hypoglycemia) is not 

good because some tissues, particularly the brain and central nervous 

system, require glucose—but too much (hyperglycemia) is also a problem. 

Glucose is chemically reactive and interacts with several biomolecules 

through the process of glycation. In the case of protein—the major 

target—glycation might inhibit biologic activity or may lead to clearance 

from the cell or circulation. You need glucose, but again, you don t have to 

ingest any—your liver makes it from protein and other compounds. 


A major point that will reappear throughout this book is that carbo¬ 

hydrate and protein can be turned to fat, but while glucose can be made 

from protein, with few exceptions, you cant make glucose from fat. In the 

context of the two fuels, this means that glucose can provide acetyl-CoA, 

but acetyl-CoA cannot be converted to glucose. 



Lipid Chemistry: Good Fats, Bad Fats 


Most of the fatty acids have common names because they were discovered 

before we had systematic chemistry and professional panels to set the 

rules. Some of the names tell you how they were discovered—for example, 

palmitic acid is found in palm oil, oleic in olives, and caproic and capryllic 

acids smell like goats (genus Capra ). 


You 11 hear a lot of disclaimers about good fats and bad fats, but in one 

way or another, the government, private agencies, and individual research¬ 

ers are still recommending that you reduce the total amount fat. The 

recommendation for reduced fat might be accompanied by an explanation 



Basic Nutrition 



77 



that the type of fat is more important than the total amount, but it usually 

boils down to some contradictory statement like: “Fat is not bad. Only 

saturated fat is bad. Eat low-fat foods.” The big targets are saturated fat, 

and of course, trans-fat. The juxtaposition of saturated fat, a natural part 

of the food chain that has existed throughout human metabolic evolution, 

and trans-fat, a by-product of industrial modification that was never seen 

before the twentieth century, is an indication that it is politics, not science, 

that is at work here. (Note that, as in figure 2.4, trans- is a general chemi¬ 

cal term, and there are naturally occurring trans-fats that might actually 

be beneficial. However that is not what is discussed here. As described 

below, trans-fats means the by-product of turning unsaturated oils into 

solid form.) 


The AHA website provides a truly maniacal cartoon video on the 

hideous Sat and Trans brothers: “They're a charming pair, Sat and Trans. 

But that doesn't mean they make good friends. Read on to learn how they 

clog arteries and break hearts—and how to limit your time with them by 

avoiding the foods they're in.'' 1 


What's missing from the website is the story behind trans-fat. The 

crusade against dietary saturated fat, which the AHA and other health 

agencies fought so vigorously, led to a search for alternatives to butter and 

lard. It is important to understand that this mission was led by physicians, 

rather than physiologists or biochemists. Some accused those in charge of 

trying to carry out a grand experiment with the American people as guinea 

pigs, but there's no stopping zealots. Butter was seen as the quintessential 

high-saturated-fat food, and it was clear that no progress could be made 

without deposing it and installing a substitute. 


Wide availability of vegetable oils provided a potential alternative to 

butter, lard, and other sources of saturated fat. Vegetable oils, however, 

are liquids, and at least for baking, have to be converted to a more useable 

form such as margarine or Crisco. You can do this through the process 

of hydrogenation: Unsaturated oils are “unsaturated” with respect to how 

much hydrogen is attached to the carbon atoms. Hydrogenation turns 

some of the unsaturated fatty acids to saturated fatty acids, in effect 

converting the oil to a solid form. Unsaturated fatty-acid molecules have 

more rigid structure and are harder to pack into a solid—which is why 

unsaturated fats tend to be liquid. Converting some of the unsaturated 



78 



Nutrition in Crisis 



fatty adds into saturated fatty acids made the material more solid and 

easier to work with. A side reaction in the manufacture of hydrogenated 

oil, however, is the conversion of some of the oil to the tram- form (the 

names refer to structure). 


As you saw in figure 2.4, double (unsaturated) bonds can be cis - or 

trans-, most naturally occurring fatty acids are cis-, so trans-fatty acids are 

not normally processed to a great extent. (Note: trans-fats are unsaturated; 

cis- or trans- can only refer to unsaturated bonds.) Some biochemists— 

notably Mary Enig—tried to stop the introduction of products containing 

trans-fatty acids, but most chemists did not know much about trans-fatty 

acids and with little support, the low-saturated-fat forces prevailed. At 

least for a while. When it turned out that trans-fat was the form that 

correlated best with cardiovascular disease, it was a ready-made scape¬ 

goat, and presumably because it was used in so many artificial products, 

it became the target of health agencies. Of course these groups did not 

mention that trans-fat in the food supply arose from their own campaign 

against saturated fat. In the end, trans-fat is a very small part of the diet, 

and its risk is probably greatly exaggerated. However there is widespread 

support for its removal among the public, and it is required by law to be 

removed. Because there’s nothing inherently good about trans-fat, nobody 

wants to defend it. 


In contrast to trans-fat, saturated fat has always been part of the 

human diet and is a normal part of metabolism. Saturated fatty acids 

are synthesized in your body through a process that is stimulated by a 

high-carbohydrate diet. This has been known for years. The process 

is called de novo lipogenesis and is in the biochemistry textbooks, and 

while it is acknowledged that high dietary carbohydrate led to de novo 

synthesis of saturated fatty acids, the idea is immediately forgotten when 

official dietary recommendations are written. So where did we get the 

idea that fat—and saturated fat, in particular—is unhealthy? Again, the 

story has been told many times, most succinctly in 7 he Rise and Fall of 

Modern Medicine , 2 most engagingly in Good Calories Bad Calories , 3 and The 

Big Fat Surprise , 4 but the death knell for the low-fat idea was, or should 

have been, the meta-analyses from several groups. 5 A collection of studies, 

some going back twenty-five years, was not able to find any risk in dietary 

saturated fat. 



Basic Nutrition 



79 



The Glucose-Insulin Axis 


It remains extremely difficult to eat a zero-carbohydrate diet. It is only 

very recently that the so-called carnivore diet has become popular. There 

are no real studies—in the absence of believable or reliable guidelines, 

self-experimentation is common. Carbohydrate is an inherent part of 

almost all human diets. There is, however, no biological requirement for 

carbohydrate in the same way there is for protein, or, to a lesser extent, 

for fat. Biologically speaking, we’ve yet to figure out whether differences 

in carbohydrate type matter, but we do know one simple principle: “High 

in carbohydrates” is bad advice for people who are overweight and espe¬ 

cially for people who have diabetes or metabolic syndrome. For many of 

them, reducing carbohydrate can constitute a cure—and while we don’t 

know for sure, it is likely that excessive carbohydrate consumption plays 

a role in how people get fat and diabetic in the first place. We have a 

grasp on the basic underlying science: Carbohydrate, directly or indi¬ 

rectly, through the hormone insulin, controls the response to other foods. 

Hormonal systems are very complex but surprisingly, in metabolism, 

insulin has an overpowering effect. It is an anabolic hormone, meaning 

it stimulates the buildup of body protein and storage of body material. 

Insulin is predominant in the storage of nutrients: It encourages the stor¬ 

age of fat and carbohydrate, and increases the synthesis of body protein. 

Hormones communicate with living cells through the docking proteins 

known as receptors, and stimulation of the receptors, in turn, triggers the 

metabolic machinery within the cell. 


Carbohydrates, especially glucose, constitute the major stimulus for 

secretion of insulin. Persistent high-insulin fluxes will bias the body 

toward storage, and particularly storage of fat. So, even though dietary fat 

is important, it plays a more passive role than is generally said. The rate at 

which fat gets stored ultimately depends on the hormones that are pres¬ 

ent. A high-fat diet with high carbohydrate is very different from a diet 

with the same amount of fat but lower carbohydrate. Higher carbohydrate, 

through its effect on insulin, might make the effect of the fat deleterious 

instead of beneficial. That’s the bottom line. 


We teach medical students that the flow of dietary fat into stored fat, 

or into the lipid markers used to characterize cardiovascular risk, is like 



80 



Nutrition in Crisis 



the flow of water through a faucet. Carbohydrate controls the faucet. If 

carbohydrate intake is low, the flow stops and fat is oxidized. In the pres¬ 

ence of high carbohydrate, on the other hand, insulin increases the rate of 

fat storage. This is simplified, of course, but not radically so. The main idea, 

that it is a control problem, not a too-much-stuff problem, is on target. 



Diet Comparisons and the Medical Literature 


Fat metabolism is only part of the picture. Insulin is a global hormone 

that regulates carbohydrate and protein metabolism. The evidence for the 

regulatory function of insulin is there, but not everybody wants to face it. 

If you confront naysayers, they tell you that the problem is very complex. 

So, how does it actually play out in the real world? It should be easy to 

get the answer. Lets consider one experiment that is pretty clear, or at 

least one that should have been clear: Bonnie Brehm et al. assigned fifty 

healthy, slightly obese women to an ad lib low-carbohydrate diet or an 

energy-restricted, low-fat diet, for four months. 6 The results were that the 

low-carbohydrate women lost significantly more weight. 


Brehm et al.’s study yielded both good and bad news. The bad news 

first: The actual spread of individual values was very large, bigger than 

what is shown in the paper. Put simply, there are different ways of showing 

variation in the data, and the method used in Brehm et al.s paper—the 

standard error of the mean (SEM)—always makes data look better than 

they are. The actual spread of values is about four times the size indicated 

in the publications. So the bad news is that outcomes on the two diets 

are not highly predictable and from the presentation of the data—group 

statistics—you cant tell who did what. 


The good news is that the big winners must have been the people in 

the low-carbohydrate group. You don’t know if most people in the low- 

carbohydrate group did better than most people in the low-fat group, or, 

alternatively, if there were a few really big winners in the low-carbohydrate 

group that tipped the scale, but you can at least be sure that low-carbohydrate 

has the possibility of the biggest payoff. 


And then there’s the really good news: If you read the Methods section, 

you’ll find the experiment followed the same protocols as Foster’s famous 

2002 study: “One group of dieters was instructed to follow an ad libitum 



Basic Nutrition 



81 



diet. , . . The other group of dieters was instructed to follow an energy- 

restricted, moderately low-fat diet with a recommended macronutrient 

distribution of 55% carbohydrate, 15% protein, and 30% fat.” 


In other words, if you were in the low-fat group, you had to count calo¬ 

ries or follow the low-calorie meal plan that they gave you, whereas if you 

were in the low-carbohydrate group, you could do whatever you wanted 

as long as you kept carbohydrates low. Even if it s a tie and the results are 

the same, most of us are going to be happier doing the low-carbohydrate 

diet since there is no restriction on calories. This is how it was done in the 

landmark Foster paper and the same pattern has continued since. 


As a scientist, you dont always read the Methods section of a paper in 

great detail unless you are planning to repeat the experiment yourself or there 

is something unusual in the way the experiment was carried out. If there is 

something important in the methodology, it should be described in the body 

of the paper. I admit that when this study was published in 2005,1 didn’t 

realize that the standard methodology was a low-calorie diet pitted against an 

ad lib low-carbohydrate diet. The rationale for this, of course, was that since 

the Atkins diet did not restrict calories, participants in the low-carbohydrate 

arm should only be instructed to reduce carbohydrate intake. This might be 

reasonable from a clinical point of view, but it does make “the diet,” rather 

than carbohydrate—which is easier to control—the independent variable. 


Although Brehm et al.’s experiment is far from the best, it is repre¬ 

sentative of the type of experiment in which the low-carbohydrate diet 

does better. However, you cannot ignore the big spread in the values. In 

2006 a meta-analysis—that is, a reexamination of previous studies—was 

performed by Alain Nordmann et al. As HI explain further in chapter 17, 

meta-analysis is a weak, possibly useless method. The idea is to average 

previous studies, but most of us agree that averaging errors makes things 

worse, not better. That said, a meta-analysis usually does give you a chance 

to see the results from several different studies. The conclusion from 

Nordmann et al.’s meta-analysis was that low-carbohydrate diets lead to 

better weight loss. Most of the studies found the low-carbohydrate diet to 

be more effective at six months but, again, it had no advantage by one year. 

The reason things got worse after six months is that the experimenters let 

them get worse: They didnt know how to keep everybody on track. When 

its your personal diet, you wont let that happen. 



82 



Nutrition in Crisis 



There are now numerous studies that present the same picture. Although 

the shorter duration studies turn out best for low-carbohydrate, you almost 

never see a study showing that low-fat is better. Unfortunately, nutritionists 

tend to consider a draw between the two diets as a win for low-fat, whereas 

when low-carbohydrate wins, the results are simply ignored, or, as in Foster s 

study, its declared that “more work needs to be done.” And so we have the 

recommendations that we do and we are in a current nutritional mess. 


A common objection to these studies is that as they depend on diet 

records—as most do—they are prone to error, and the subjects in the 

studies often misreport what they eat. Errors in reporting have been 

documented, but the data are not completely inaccurate—usually they 

are about 80 percent accurate. All experiments have error, however. It is 

only a question of how you deal with the error. For inaccuracy in dietary 

reporting to account for the difference between diets it would be necessary 

for subjects in the low-fat group to underreport what they ate and for the 

low-carbohydrate people to overreport what they ate, or both. These errors 

are certainly possible, but again, from a practical standpoint, it might be 

good to be on a diet where you think you ate more than you actually did. 



Animal Models, Human Subjects 


To be fair to the low-fat doctrine, it is easy to be misled. Animal studies 

are critical in biological science and it seems that mice, especially those 

bred for laboratory work, will get fat on high-fat diets even without any 

carbohydrate. 7 How is that possible? People dont usually get fat on high- 

fat diets, or at least they dont overconsume fat to such a high degree unless 

their diet is also high in carbohydrate. Mice provide a good model for 

human metabolism in other cases, too, so this is a serious difference to 

consider. While carbohydrate is key in human metabolism, the same is not 

necessarily so in rodent metabolism, where a high-fat diet can bring on 

obesity, diabetes, and cardiovascular disease even in the absence of carbo¬ 

hydrate. We dont yet have a theory to encompass the differences between 

animal models and human subjects. If our understanding of the catalytic 

role of insulin is correct, however, then it might well be that mice (who 

normally live on high-carbohydrate diets) maintain a functionally high 

level of insulin all the time. In other words, the bias toward an anabolic 



Basic Nutrition 



83 



state that occurs with high-carbohydrate diets in humans might always be 

“on” in mice. 


Whatever the explanation, its hard for many to recognize that the 

animal model system we have used so extensively to understand humans 

might actually have the most impact precisely because there is a difference 

between how people and mice respond to certain treatments. In this case, 

animal models might offer a clue to human behavior and physiological 

response through its differences from that of the mice. 


The real problem, though, is that we have not faced the results of the 

practical, experimental tests in humans. We have large, expensive clinical 

trials with a consistent and reliable outcome: There is no effect of dietary 

fat on obesity, cardiovascular disease, or just about anything else. The 

unwillingness to face these failures makes this a remarkable phenomenon 

in the history of medicine—that it persists in a period of sophisticated 

science and technology makes it nearly unbelievable. Unbelievable is the 

keyword. One understands that science can be incomplete or might have 

flaws, but it is hard to understand how the whole establishment could 

maintain such a misguided opinion on the diet-heart hypothesis. How 

can they keep doing the same experiment over and over without success? 

How can they get away with it, and just as importantly, why would they 

want to get away with it? 


In science, excluding a theory is always easier than showing consistency. 

In this case, if the fat-cholesterol-heart connection was as inescapable a 

risk as they make it out to be, then none of these big studies should fail. Not 

one. In fact, almost all fail. Yes, there have been increasing admissions that 

high-carbohydrate is not a good thing, but these admissions usually come 

with a qualifier such as “especially refined sugar” or “particularly refined 

starch,” despite the fact that no study has directly compared “refined” and 

“unrefined” carbohydrates (high-GI and low-GI are not measures of refine¬ 

ment and they are, in any case, weak predictors.) The drastic increase in total 

carbohydrate that has accompanied the obesity epidemic is its most salient 

feature. Some may choose to ignore it if they don’t like it, but it is there. 


So, does this mean we can add more fat to our diets, or is fat still bad? 

Its probably fair to say that most people think that in some way fat it still 

bad, but the role of fat in the body is itself controlled—directly or indi¬ 

rectly through hormones—by carbohydrate. Deleterious effects of lipid 



84 



Nutrition in Crisis 



metabolism are ultimately dependent on carbohydrate intake. Perhaps 

most surprising, the biochemistry shows that although it is the fatty acids 

in your blood that are the problem, they are more likely to come from 

dietary carbohydrate than from dietary fat. 


Is Carbohydrate Fattening? 


I don t understand. I went to this conference and they had a 

buffet every night , and I really pigged out on roast beef and 

lobster> but I didn't gain any weight 


—-Jeffrey Feinman, the authors brother 


Nobody manages to avoid weight gain after going on a cruise and pigging 

out on pasta, but the same isn’t true for those, like my brother, who indulge 

in roast beef and lobster. To return once again to the faucet analogy: Insulin 

opens the faucet for fat storage but it shuts down the faucet for fat oxidation. 

At this point, you might ask whether these details matter. Doesn’t it all even 

out in the end? Isn’t it just calories in, calories out, or, as they always say in the 

news releases, “a calorie is a calorie”—and don’t the laws of thermodynamics 

tell us that? It is hard to tell the extent to which you can lose more weight, 

calorie for calorie, by changing the composition of the diet. One clue, though, 

is that when experiments show that one macronutrient is less efficient than 

another (wastes calories as heat), it is usually the low-carbohydrate arm that 

is less fattening. Critics say that these results are due to inaccurate reporting 

of food intake, and that it is always just a matter of total calories consumed. 

Low-carbohydrate diets, they claim, simply reduce total energy intake. It’s 

true that food frequency records can have substantial error, as in the Brehm 

study. If reduced calorie intake is indeed why the low-carbohydrate group 

always wins in these face-offs, then as we know, low-carbohydrate partici¬ 

pants would have to be overreporting what they ate or low-fat comparisons 

groups would have to be underreporting what they ate, or both. Wouldn’t 

this be a positive point for low-carbohydrate though? There might be a real 

benefit to being on a diet where you think you ate more than you did. After 

all, there is no “just” about reducing calories. 


The other hole in the critics’argument is that thermodynamics does not 

predict that “a calorie is a calorie.” Most people who quote the “la>vs of 



Basic Nutrition 



85 



thermodynamics” (they usually mean just the first law) have never studied 

thermodynamics. The essential feature of thermodynamics rests not with 

the first law, which is about energy conservation, but rather with the second 

law, which says that all (real) processes are inefficient. Energy is dissipated. 

The variable efficiency (the extent to which energy is wasted as heat) of fat, 

protein, and carbohydrate is well known, but in the medical literature, it is 

ignored at will. In cases where total calories turn out to be the controlling 

variable, independent of macronutrient composition, it is because of the 

homeostatic (stabilizing) mechanisms of biological systems, not because of 

thermodynamics. Thermodynamics is my special interest, and we’ll come 

back to the subject in chapter 9. 


“The Atkins Diet Is a 

High-Calorie Starvation Diet” 


This quotation is from George Cahill, one of the pioneers in the study 

of metabolism and the response to starvation. The idea is that the 

reduction in blood glucose and insulin and the increase in glucagon 

that accompany lower carbohydrate intake resemble the changes that 

are associated with total reduction in calories. In starvation, insulin goes 

down, glucagon goes up, fat oxidation increases, and—at some point— 

ketone bodies are generated. 


In 1992, Klein and Wolfe carried out a defining experiment. 8 The 

subjects went without food for three days, were given a period of rest, and 

then went through another three days of starvation. During the second 

stretch without food, however, the subjects received intravenous injection 

of a lipid emulsion (fat) that was designed to meet their resting energy 

requirements. The first period represented a model of early starvation, and 

the second represented an absence of food intake with adequate energy. 

Klein and Wolfe measured several physiologic parameters during each 

period, and as shown in table 4.1, there was not a great difference between 

the two periods despite the very large discrepancy in energy intake. The 

levels of free fatty acids were expected to be different—in the first period, 

breakdown of body fat was required for energy, so free fatty acids would 

be high; and in the second period, free fatty acids would be lower since 

injected lipid would be used for energy—but, in fact, the values were the 



86 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Table 4.1. Similarity of Starvation and Carbohydrate Restriction 

Free 


fatty adds Fat oxidation Glucose (l-Hydroxybutyrate 

(pmol/I) (pmol/kg/min] (mg/dl] ImM) 


84-hour fast .92 1.94 68 2.56 


84-hour fast 1.02 1.67 66 2.54 


+ lipid 


Source S. Klein and R R Wolf, "Carbohydrate restriction regulates the adaptive 


response to fasti ng”4meffcan journal of Physiology 262, no 5 [1992]: E631-636 


same. Similarly, ketone bodies, which reflect the absence of calories, were 

reasonably high in the fasting group, but in the second case, with adequate 

energy, one might have expected low ketone bodies. The explanation of 

the experiment is that the controlling factor was the level of insulin and 

glucose. The study concluded that “these results demonstrate that restric¬ 

tion of dietary carbohydrate, not the general absence of energy intake itself, 

is responsible for initiation of the metabolic response to short-term fast¬ 

ing.” The statement is undoubtedly something of an exaggeration—there 

are other factors that might have modified the results—but the experiment 

brings out one of the major themes in diet and metabolism: Carbohydrate 

is a controlling element, whereas dietary fat plays a relatively passive role. 

Circulating fat, body fat, and fatty acids do play a role in metabolism, but 

it is wrong to assume that dietary fat equates to body lipids. This is a major 

theme in this book: “You are what you eat” is not a good principle. 



Looking Back, Are Carbohydrates Fattening? 


Harpers Illustrated Biochemistry is one of the standard texts in medical and 

graduate schools. Now in its twenty-ninth edition, it is a multi-authored 

comprehensive view of the field. I am grateful to Adele Hite of the 

University of North Carolina for pointing out that in the eighth edition 

of the text, published in 1961 when it was called Review of Physiological 

Chemistry and Harper himself was the sole author, the close connection 

between carbohydrate and fat was evident. The chapter on metabolism of 

carbohydrate began as follows: “In the average diet carbohydrate compro¬ 

mises more than half of the total caloric intake. However, only a limited 



Basic Nutrition 



87 



Going Without Food 


Human metabolism has two goals: to provide energy and to 

maintain blood glucose at a relatively constant level. In the 

eight hours or so after a meal—the fed state, or what nutri¬ 

tionists call the postprandial state—diet can provide a greater 

or lesser amount of the material needed to meet these two 

needs. If you go long enough without eating, food no longer 

provides material for metabolism directly—this is referred to 

as fasting, or the postabsorptive state. When you wake up in 

the morning after an overnight fast, insulin is low and glucagon 

is high, so fat is broken down through the process of lipolvsis 

into fatty acids and glycerol (lipolysis is inhibited by insulin and 

stimulated by glucagon.) The fatty acids are oxidized for energy, 

and blood glucose is maintained by the processing of liver 

glycogen. Muscle also stores glycogen that can be broken down. 

This glucose is used by the muscle itself and is not exported. 

In simpler terms, as noted before, we can think of muscle as a 

consumer of glucose and the liver as a supplier, or more gener¬ 

ally, as a command center for metabolism. 


Gluconeogenesis 


Gluconeogenesis is frequently described as a last-ditch 

source of energy when food isn’t avail able and glycogen is 

depicted, but in fact, gluconeogenesis is happening all the 

time. When you wake up in the morning, more than half of 

the free glucose in the blood or produced from previously 

stored glycogen comes from gluconeogenesis. Although there 

is no form of protein formally defined as a storage site, as 

there is for stored fat or glycogen, muscle can be thought of 

as providing an internal source of protein, a source that must 

be replenished from the diet, a dynamic store of amino acids 

for metabolism. 



88 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Is Starvation a Good Way to Lose Weight? 


In the fasting state, adipocytes (fat cells) supply fatty acids from 

the breakdown of fat. Most tissues, including the heart, oxidize 

the fatty acids for energy. Some tissues—primarily the bxain and 

central nervous system—require glucose, which can be provided 

by tbe combination of glycogen breakdown and replenishment 

from glueoneogenesis. The two goals of metabolism are thus 

taken care of. Is starvation a good way to lose weight? Of course 

it is not. The requirement for amino acids from protein for the 

maintenance of blood glucose is the problem. In the absence of 

dietary protein, your body will turn to its own sources. Fasting 

has become popular, but the breakdown of protein after a day or 

two might have physiologic consequences. The answer is not in! 

so I would be cautious of a fast for more than twenty-four hours. 



amount of this dietary carbohydrate can be stored as such. It is now known 

that the un-stored portion of the ingested carbohydrate is converted to fat 

by the metabolic processes of lipogenesis.” 9 


In other words, the third sentence of the carbohydrate section of a 

biochemistry text emphasized the closeness of carbohydrate and fat. In the 

twentieth edition (1985), the chapter, now written by Peter Mayes, begins 

similarly and continues: “It is possible that in humans the frequency of 

taking meals and the extent to which carbohydrates are converted to fat 

could have a bearing on disease states such as atherosclerosis, obesity and 

diabetes mellitus.” 10 


In the current edition, the process of conversion of carbohydrate to fat 

now has a chapter of its own. The process is known as de novo lipogenesis, 

new synthesis of fat, or more precisely de novo fatty-acid synthesis, since 

the immediate product is a fatty acid, the saturated fatty acid palmitic acid 

(C16:0). De novo lipogenesis appears to be the explanation of the counter¬ 

intuitive result, demonstrated in several studies, that dietary carbohydrate 

leads to increases in saturated fatty acids in the blood. Chapter 9 describes 



Basic Nutrition 



89 



an experiment from Jeff Volek, then at the University of Connecticut, where 

such an increase in saturated fatty acids was greater in the blood of people 

on a high-carbohydrate diet compared to those on a low-carbohydrate diet, 

even though the latter had three times the amount of dietary saturated fat. 



What About Protein? 


In some classes that I teach, I ask the students for the definition of life. I 

get different answers but I usually say “No, a one-word definition.” I try not 

to drag it out too long or to overact, but the answer that I am looking for 

is “protein.” Everything that goes on in life is controlled by protein, either 

as the actual component or as the source for other things. Because of its 

multiple roles in biology and the far more complicated chemistry, I will 

present here only broad outlines in the context of an answer to an email. 


I received the following question: 


If one is on a very-low-carbohydrate/high-fat diet, what 

happens to excess protein that is not needed for muscle repair 

and growth, and gluconeogenesis? I see two alternatives: 


1. Its excreted 


2. More glucose is created. 


Number 2 seems so unreasonable to me. Would your metabo¬ 

lism actually make more than the little bit it needs? Fm open to 

a number 3 that I might be too unimaginative to think of Fm 

interested in the theory. This isnt a request for diet advice. I like 

to understand things at the cellular level. 


The answer is that it depends on what else is going on, but protein, per 

se, is not excreted (in the absence of some disease). Protein is a polymer. 

Unlike glycogen, which is a homopolymer of identical glucose units, the 

individual units, amino acids, are picked from about twenty different 

choices. In digestion, protein is broken down to individual amino acids, 

which are absorbed and reassembled into body protein. The sequence of 

amino acids defines the biologic function, and this sequence is encoded 



90 



Nutrition in Crisis 



in the genetic material. The genetic code is largely the code of amino 

acid sequences. 


After digestion and absorption, some of the amino acids that are not 

used for protein synthesis may be trashed. The nitrogen is converted to 

ammonia. Ammonia is converted to the compound urea and excreted. The 

remaining carbon skeleton can be used for energy either directly in the 

citric acid (TCA) cycle or by conversion to ketone bodies, especially on a 

very low-carbohydrate diet. Some amino acids can be converted to glucose. 

Much more than a little bit is needed. The carbon skeleton from amino 

acids, directly or indirectly, can be converted to fat. So a practical answer is 

that “excess” protein is recycled: used for energy or for synthesis of glucose. 


Protein, as such, is not normally excreted. Proteinuria is an indica¬ 

tion of some abnormality, kidney malfunction, or other disease-related 

nephropathy. Amino acids are excreted at some low level. High excretion 

of particular amino acids is usually an indication of some metabolic distur¬ 

bance or inborn error of metabolism. It is important to understand that 

everything that goes on in the body is mediated by proteins, which turn 

over all the time, and whereas muscle “repair and growth” is important, it is 

not the only thing. Body proteins, unlike glycogen or other homopolymers, 

have specific amino acid sequences and so require a particular makeup. 

Some amino acids are interconverted and some (essential amino acids) are 

required from diet. 


Current tendencies are to try to encourage vegetarianism or, at least, 

to encourage reduction of meat consumption. Whatever the moral or 

practical arguments are, the scientific case is highly questionable. The 

proliferating studies trying to demonstrate an association between meat 

consumption and one disease or another are largely bogus, and a couple 

of these studies are deconstructed in chapter 14. The past few years have 

also seen a proliferation of “experts” whose training and expertise in nutri¬ 

tion were still being questioned and who have now become authorities on 

global warming and sustainability of life itself. 




-CHAPTER 5- 



An Introduction 

to Metabolism 


T he nineteenth century was a period of political, intellectual, and 

scientific revolution. Although the politics made more noise, 

there might be greater long-term impact from the turnarounds 

in intellectual and scientific fields. Paris in the summer of 1848 was the 

site of yet another French Revolution. People had taken to the streets and 

were building barricades just as in Les Mis. There was dissatisfaction over 

rights of assembly and the autocratic government, but rising food prices 

were perhaps the larger driving force. Whatever the origin of the disor¬ 

der, faculty at the College de France complained that it had “slackened 

the zeal for research among all of the chemists,” and that their time was 

“absorbed by politics.” 1 The intellectual revolution at the College, however, 

was unfolding in Claude Bernard's laboratory. 


Bernard, generally considered the father of modern physiology, had 

been studying digestion in dogs. He found sugar in a dog that he had been 

dissecting, despite the fact that the animal had not been fed any sugar. The 

finding was revolutionary because it was generally assumed at the time that 

any sugar in an animal had to have come from the diet. Furthermore, it was 

expected that even if an animal had consumed sugar, that sugar would, in 

the end, be destroyed by oxidation. Antoine Lavoisier had shown more than 

one hundred years before that animals eat sugar—or any food—for energy, 

and that the energy comes from oxidation of the food, just as if you were 

burning food in a furnace. So how did sugar wind up undigested in the 

animal? Bernard s first thought was that there might be something wrong 

with the reagent that he had used to detect the sugar, but the reagent was 

okay—the dog really was making its own sugar. In fact, he soon found that 

if he fed a dog only meat, there was as much sugar in that animal as there 



92 



Nutrition in Crisis 



was in another dog that had been fed “sugary soup.” Strange as it seemed, he 

concluded that the dog must have been making its own sugar. 


The Discovery of Glycogen and Gluconeogenesis 


Although Bernard's experimental findings were occasionally 

at fault and at times influenced by preconceptions , . . his 

strength appears to lie in his ability to discard a theory once its 

experimental basis had been undermined. Even though he was 

apt not to state frankly that he had been wrong, he neverthe¬ 

less did change his ideas. 


—F. G. Young 2 


It took some further work to show that the sugar was actually being 

produced in the dogs liver, but by 1857, Claude Bernard had isolated 

the matiere glycogene —that is, glycogen. We know now that glycogen is 

a storage form of carbohydrate, a polymer of glucose. It is actually made 

from glucose, although the glucose that goes into glycogen may come from 

something else. 


As I explained in chapter 4, glycogen is a highly branched, highly struc¬ 

tured polymer, and it is the key player in maintaining blood glucose at a 

constant level. When cells use up the available glucose, the liver breaks 

down glycogen to reestablish a constant level. Bernard emphasized this role 

of glycogen as a supplier, providing glucose to the circulation, but he was 

not quite right about how glycogen formed in the first place. He knew that 

glycogen didn't come from fat, and he found through further experimenta¬ 

tion that dietary protein seemed to raise glycogen levels even more than 

dietary sugar (he used fibrin, the blood coagulation protein for his tests). 

The picture that evolved was that protein was the source of glycogen, which, 

in turn, could produce glucose. This was not exactly right—if carbohydrate 

intake is high, ingested sugar is converted to glycogen—but Bernard had 

discovered something critical: the need for a process that converted other 

things into glucose, a process now known as gluconeogenesis that would 

not be fully understood for another hundred years. 


Our current understanding focuses on glycogen synthesis and breakdown 

as a control point in metabolism. Sugar in the diet or in the circulation can 



An Introduction to Metabolism 



93 



be stored in glycogen and then made available when needed. However, it 

is not only dietary glucose that shows up in glycogen. Bernard was right 

that sugar could be made in the liver from protein—that is, amino acids 

from proteins.The glucose synthesized in gluconeogenesis can be exported 

to the blood, or alternatively, can be used to replenish previously used 

glycogen, and the new glucose molecules might only appear in the blood 

at a later time. 


Although Bernard understood that glucose came from glycogen, he did 

not realize that glycogen could also be made from ingested sugar. The key 

mistake was Bernard’s inability to grasp that glucose is present in the blood 

all the time and it is maintained at a constant level. He didn’t get it because 

he had actually made an experimental error. A lucky one, it turns out. 


Sometimes We Luck Out 


Bernard’s reason for believing that glycogen was not made from glucose 

came from his measurements of the inputs to and outputs from the liver. 

Bernard determined the amount of sugar in the portal vein, which brings 

blood from the digestive tract to the liver (its not a true vein), and he 

also measured the amount of glucose leaving the liver in the hepatic veins. 

His original observations showed that there was little or no glucose in the 

inputs from the portal vein, but that there was sugar in the hepatic veins 

leaving the liver. In other words, he could see glucose exiting the liver but 

no glucose coming in. This observation pretty much made his case that the 

liver was a sugar-producing organ and that glycogen was being made from 

something else. 


Some of Bernards scientific rivals said that he was wrong, claiming that 

they had been able to detect sugar coming into the liver in the portal vein. 

They were right, of course—we know that there is sugar throughout the 

circulation and that glycogen is assembled from blood glucose. Bernard 

had made the mistake of letting some of his preparations sit around too 

long while the sugar in the portal vein to the liver was being metabolized. 

Bernard was right to an extent: Usually, more sugar is leaving the liver than is 

coming in (the fiver does produce glucose), but the differences are small and 

the instruments available to Bernard might not have allowed him to detect 

them. It was this error that allowed him to piece together a picture of a more 



94 



Nutrition in Crisis 



complicated part of metabolism. Although he got things slightly wrong and 

had to modify his ideas later, the error allowed him to identify glycogen and 

to generate the idea of gluconeogenesis. Sometimes we luck out. 


Today, we emphasize the need to keep blood glucose constant. We 

understand that almost all cells in the body can use glucose as an energy 

source, so having too little is not good. However, it turns out that too 

much is also not good, because glucose will react with proteins in the blood 

and tissues to form what are called advanced glycation end-products, or 

AGEs. These modified proteins can be a factor in aging and degenerative 

disease. In the popular press and on social media, the negative effects of 

high glucose have led to the idea that glucose is a toxin, which is surely 

exaggerated, or at least a mischaracterization of the AGEs. 


An Evolving Understanding of Metabolism 


Through the work of Bernard and others—particularly Louis Pasteur—the 

end of the nineteenth century saw the evolution of a science of metabolism, 

allowing greater understanding of the inner workings of the body and how 

sources of energy are used. By that point, we knew that blood glucose came 

from the liver as well as from the diet, and that dietary glucose came from 

sugars and starches. 


Looking ahead, under conditions where there is no food or there is low 

dietary carbohydrate, there will be a continuing drain on body protein. 

In the latter case, protein for the body can be supplied from the diet, but 

ketone bodies provide an alternative source of energy to reduce the depen¬ 

dence on protein. Ketone body synthesis and utilization interact, as one 

would expect, with glycogen metabolism. 


To put this in context, the next section will provide an overview of 

energy metabolism, illustrating the principle that, in metabolism, there are 

two goals and two fuels. 


The Black Box of Life 


Metabolism—the conversion of food to energy and cell materials—is as 

complicated as you would expect, but it is possible to get an idea of the 

big picture. The approach here is called the “black box” strategy: getting 



An Introduction to Metabolism 



95 




Figure 5.1. The black box approach to metabolism. A, The black box of life summarizes 

how we eat food, take in oxygen, and excrete C0 2 and water. B, Inside the black box, 

we see the oxidation of food is carried out by an intermediate coenzyme NAD+. The 

product reduces oxygen to water. C, The main process. The big players. 



as much information as possible by looking at the inputs and outputs 

of a system without necessarily knowing the details of what’s going on 

inside. As we uncover more details, we can nest black boxes inside each 

other, thereby organizing the limited information we have. This method is 

favored by engineers, who are the people most unhappy with the idea that 

they don’t know anything at all. 


You likely already have a basic idea of what we do in metabolism: We take 

in food and oxygen, and put out CO 2 and water. Looking at the inputs and 

outputs (see figure 5.1), even without knowing a great deal about chemis¬ 

try, you can figure out that oxidation is occurring within the box—like the 

burning of fuel to generate heat or run a machine. Technically speaking, 

it is an oxidation-reduction reaction (redox, for short). Oxidation, in this 

context, means combination with oxygen. In metabolism, it is frequently 

the hydrogen atom attached to a metabolite that is oxidized (to water). 












96 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Chemical Energy 


In physics, energy refers to the ability to do work. As compli¬ 

cated as systems can get, we are basically talking about lifting 

a weight on a pulley In chemistry, energy is identified with the 

progress of a reaction. If a chemical reaction proceeds by itself, 

at any speed, without the addition of work, we know that we 

can use it to lift a weight. If you have studied any chemistry, 

you will remember the equilibrium constant, which tells you 

how much product you have at the end of the reaction. If the 

constant is favorable—that is, if you have a lot of product—then 

the reaction is said to be exergonic, downhill and spontaneous. 

You can get energy from it. 



There are two parts to an oxidation-reduction reaction. In the oxidation of 

a hydrogen atom (alone or in a compound), the oxygen is said to be reduced. 

The generalization used in biochemistry is that a compound gets oxidized if 

it combines with oxygen, and becomes reduced if it combines with hydrogen. 

(Redox reactions are a fundamental part of all chemistry and the concepts 

have been highly developed, but this simplification works well in biochem¬ 

istry.) Like combustion reactions, redox reactions produce energy that can 

then be used to do work. Some energy is used for mechanical work—moving 

muscles—but most goes toward chemical work: making body material, 

keeping biological structures intact, generally keeping things running. 



The Two Goals 


To review, there are two major goals in human metabolism: first, provide 

energy for life processes, and second, maintain more or less constant levels 

of blood glucose. Too little glucose is not good because it is a major fuel, 

but too much is also harmful because it will react with proteins to create 

AGEs, as described earlier in this chapter. 



An Introduction to Metabolism 



97 



Energy in biochemistry is described in terms of a particular chemi¬ 

cal reaction. When you study biochemistry, you first examine what the 

compounds do precisely, but then you use them as abbreviations. Phosphate 

ion (unattached to other atoms) is abbreviated Pi, where the “i” stands for 

“inorganic.” Although it is now considered somewhat archaic, Pi is still 

sometimes read as “inorganic phosphate.” So, the big energy system in 

biology is: 



ADP + Pi ATP + H 2 0 (1) 


Energy storage occurs in living systems through the synthesis of 

ATP from ADP (the other reactants are assumed). In metabolism, you 

need “energy” from food to make ATP from ADP. Then, when ATP 

is converted back to the low-energy form, ADP, through hydrolysis 

(adding water)—that is, when the above equation moves from right to 

left—energy is released. This energy can be used to do chemical work, 

and make proteins, DNA, and other metabolites and cell material. The 

reaction is favored in the reverse direction: It tends toward the release 

of energy. 


Textbooks frequently refer to ATP as a “high-energy molecule,” but it 

is the reaction (synthesis and hydrolysis), rather than the compound itself, 

that is high energy. For the moment, we can think of ATP as the “coin of 

energy exchange in metabolism” and the ATP to ADP ratio as the energy 

state of the system. 



The Two Fuels 


In order to fulfill the two goals, two kinds of fuels are used: glucose itself 

and the two-carbon compound acetyl-coenzyme A (abbreviated acetyl- 

CoA or acetyl-SCoA). Coenzyme A is a complicated molecule, but its 

not important to know the details for our purposes. Coenzymes are small 

molecules that take part in the metabolic changes in living systems. They 

can be involved in energy metabolism (such as ATP to ADP) or other 

reactions. The oxidation-reduction coenzymes are the NAD (nicotinamide 

adenine dinucleotide) molecules, of which there are two forms: oxidized, 

NAD+, and reduced, NADH. 



98 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Most ATP in the cell comes from the oxidation of acetyl-CoA, but 

glucose can be converted to acetyl-CoA. Acetyl-CoA also comes from 

fat, and to a smaller extent, from protein. Glucose itself can be formed 

from protein but not from acetyl-CoA. The significance of this, as we 

have said, is that fat can be formed from glucose, but with a few minor 

exceptions, glucose cannot be formed from fat. Historically, the challenge 

for biochemistry has been to explain how the energy from an oxidation- 

reduction reaction could be used to carry out the synthesis of ATP, which 

has a different mechanism (phosphate transfer). The process is called 

oxidative phosphorylation and was only figured out about fifty years ago. 


Breaking into the black box, oxidation of food is separated into two 

different processes. The food never sees the oxygen but instead there is an 

intermediary player. The intermediate agent, the redox coenzyme NAD+, 

does the oxidation of food, and the NADH (the product, the reduced 

form) is re-oxidized by molecular oxygen. Why do we do it this way? In 

general, biochemical reactions proceed in small steps to allow for control 

and for capturing energy. Even if we could do it all in one big blast, like 

an automobile engine, living tissues do not do well with explosive, high- 

temperature reactions—we would have little control over them and we 

would not be able to capture the energy in a usable chemical form. 


Glycolysis 


Glucose is at the center of metabolism. Glycolysis, the collection of early 

steps in its processing, is common to almost all organisms. Glycolysis (sugar 

splitting) ultimately provides two molecules of the three-carbon compound 

known as pyruvic acid (pyruvate). In most cells, the pyruvate from glycolysis 

is oxidized to acetyl-CoA, which is the input for aerobic (oxygen-based) 

metabolism and the main source of energy for most mammalian cells. One 

of the functions of glycolysis is to prepare glucose for oxidative metabolism— 

that is, to provide acetyl-CoA. Some cells, however, can run on glycolysis 

alone. Such cells are said to have a glycolytic metabolism and can convert 

pyruvate to a number of different compounds, most commonly lactic acid. 


Many microorganisms are glycolytic, and much of our understanding of 

glycolysis comes from the study of bacteria and the process of fermentation. 

The final product of glycolysis can be very different from one organism to 



An Introduction to Metabolism 



99 



another. Alcoholic fermentation involves the conversion of pyruvate to a 

two-carbon compound acetaldehyde, which in turn is converted to ethanol. 

(When you ingest alcohol, your liver runs this reaction backward, converting 

alcohol to acetaldehyde and then to acetyl-CoA, which is further oxidized.) 

Other kinds of glycolytic bacteria, like those in yogurt, convert pyruvate to 

lactic acid (lactate), accounting for the acidity of yogurt Mammalian cells can 

also carry out this transformation. The brain, central nervous system, red blood 

cells, and rapidly exercising muscle are the most common of the glycolytic 

tissues that produce lactate. It was once believed that the lactate produced by 

exercising muscle was the cause of delayed-onset muscle soreness (DOMS), 

but this is not true. Although the cause of DOMS is not known, the lactic 

acid is metabolized and long gone by the time soreness sets in. 



Oxidative Metabolism 


Acetyl-CoA is the main substrate for the oxidative process that produces 

CO 2 , which is then released from the black box of life. This process is 

frequently called the Krebs cycle, after Sir Hans Krebs, who was the 

pioneer in assembling a coherent mechanism from the various observa¬ 

tions of where particular carbon atoms went when different foods were fed 

to a tissue or organism. Oxidation of such a small molecule as acetyl-CoA 

would have to take place in a small number of steps, and would not allow 

the kind of control that it is necessary to keep a biological system respon¬ 

sive to different conditions, so the two carbons of acetyl-CoA are attached 

to a carrier to form a six-carbon molecule, called citric acid, or citrate. 

For this reason the cycle is frequendy referred to as the citric acid cycle. 

Complicating things further, citric acid is a tricarboxylic acid, or TCA for 

short, so the process might also be called the TCA cycle. All three names 

are used: the Krebs cycle, the citric acid cycle, and the TCA cycle. Krebs 

himself called it the TCA cycle, so we will try to stick with that. 


The TCA cycle is complicated, but I will try to provide a rough descrip¬ 

tion: The substrate, acetyl-CoA, is bound to a carrier to form a compound, 

citric acid, that is oxidized, stepwise, primarily by the redox coenzyme 

NAD+.The products of the reaction are CO 2 and the reduced form of the 

coenzyme, NADH. NADH is the ultimate reducing agent (transfers H) 

that turns oxygen to water. This process, the electron transport chain, is a 



100 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Sources of Acetyl-CoA 


Glycolysis is not the only source of acetyl-C oA for the oxida¬ 

tive metabolism of glucose. The major source, for many cells, 

is fatty acids from fat. The process of converting fatty acids is 

called (3-oxidation. The long-chain fatty' acids are chopped two 

at a time from the carboxyl end. It uses the Greek character (3 

because the break is at the second carbon in the fatty acid chain. 



sequence of reactions that, in effect, transfers electrons. The net effect is 

the reduction of molecular oxygen to water. Somehow this converts ADP 

to ATP. This is the mechanism for channeling the energy of burning food 

into the conversion of ATP back to ADP—that is, the storing of chemical 

energy. The process, still mysterious when I was in graduate school, is now 

understood. The idea behind it is called the chemiosmotic theory—really 

no longer a theory—which was largely the work of a single man, Peter 

Mitchell. It would be a major digression to delve into the overall process 

here, but I recommend doing some research into both chemiosmotic 

theory and Peter Mitchell himself. Because it involves oxygen, obtaining 

energy from the TCA cycle electron transport chain is referred to as oxida¬ 

tive metabolism, distinct from glycolytic metabolism, which might precede 

it. Glycolysis provides acetyl-CoA for the TCA cycle. Looking ahead, a 

major feature of cancer cells, referred to as the Warburg effect, appears 

to be a greater reliance on glycolysis rather than oxidative metabolism, in 

comparison to normal cells, even when oxygen is present. Explaining the 

Warburg effect is a major focus of current cancer research. 



Ketone Bodies 


Ketone bodies have evolved as an alternative energy source under condi¬ 

tions of starvation or carbohydrate restriction. Synthesis and utilization 

of ketone bodies provide a way of dealing with the biochemical principle 



An Introduction to Metabolism 



101 



stated so many times throughout this book: You can make fat from glucose 

but you cannot make glucose from fat. None of the energy stored in the 

form of fat will help in providing glucose for the brain and the central 

nervous system (CNS). In the absence of dietary carbohydrate or total 

calories, protein becomes the main source of carbons for gluconeogen- 

esis—and the risk here is that you will break down essential body protein 

to meet this second goal of metabolism, maintaining blood glucose. 


Created in the liver from acetyl-CoA, the ketone bodies, 

p-hydroxybutyrate and acetoacetate, are four-carbon compounds. They 

provide a way for acetyl-CoA units (from fat) to be transported from the 

liver to other tissues, where they are turned back into acetyl-CoA and used 

for energy. Through this process, protein no longer has to bear the full 

pressure of providing glucose under extreme conditions. 


The brain and CNS cannot use fatty acids for fuel. These and some 

other tissues are dependent on glucose, at least under normal well-fed 

conditions. The dilemma in a starvation state, or on a low-carbohydrate 

diet, is how to supply energy to these tissues. Because you cannot make 

glucose from fat, amino acids from protein must supply glucose via gluco- 

neogenesis. In starvation, that protein must come from muscle and other 

body proteins. It is the demand for glucose, rather than total energy, that 

is the problem under extreme conditions. (On a low-carbohydrate diet, the 

problem is avoided because of the availability of dietary protein. While 

low-carbohydrate diets are not necessarily high in protein, a somewhat 

higher level is needed to supply material for gluconeogenesis.) 


The ketone bodies are derived from fatty acids, but unlike fatty acids them¬ 

selves, they can be used by the brain and CNS because they supply acetyl-CoA 

directly. The evolutionary advantage of ketone bodies is that they provide a 

way to avoid the breakdown of body muscle stores. For this reason, ketosis 

(ketone bodies in the blood) is generally described as “protein-sparing/Tn the 

beginning stages of ketosis, muscle tends to get most of the ketone bodies for 

fuel, but as things proceed, more ketone bodies are diverted to the brain. The 

ketone bodies can reduce the need for glucose by more than half. 


A low-carbohydrate diet will supply the protein for glucose synthesis, but 

the adaptive mechanisms that have evolved to spare protein in starvation 

will remain operative—so the state of the body with reduced carbohydrate 

intake is not unlike that in starvation. This is why George Cahill, who did 



102 



Nutrition in Crisis 



the pioneering work in ketone bodies and starvation, described the Atkins 

diet as a “high-calorie starvation diet.” 


There is no requirement for dietary glucose, but what is the requirement 

for glucose in the body as a fuel? The number that you see in the literature is 

130 grams per day, and it’s a number with a strange history. In George Cahills 

classic study on the response to starvation, 3 it was found that this was the 

amount of glucose consumed by the brain under normal conditions—that 

is, before the starvation phase of the experiment began. After several days 

of starvation, however, the amount was found to be substantially less, in the 

range of 50 grams per day (at this point, the glucose was obviously not coming 

from diet, since it was measured under starvation conditions). Somehow, 

nutritionists picked up on the baseline 130 grams per day figure and even 

morphed this into a dietary requirement. Cahill told my colleague Eugene 

Fine that by the time he realized this had happened it was too late to stop it. 

The mistake has since spread throughout the literature, compounded by the 

suggestion that not only do you need 130 grams of glucose (not always true), 

but that you need to get that 130 grams specifically from diet (never tme). 


In summary, the brain and CNS require about 50 grams per day of 

glucose, and under conditions of low glucose, other fuels—namely, ketone 

bodies—might become more important. Many questions remain unan¬ 

swered, however. Who is directing all this? How does the fat cell know when 

to provide fatty acids to the liver? How does the liver know when to make 

ketone bodies, and how many? What controls whether glucose is burned 

for energy or stored as fat? And thus we see the real problem in the study 

of metabolism. A metabolic map, like any map, only tells you about possible 

routes; it doesn’t tell you where the traffic lights are, and it might or might 

not tell you where the traffic cops are. What we do know is that the most 

important of the traffic regulators, not surprisingly at this point, is insulin. 



The Role of Insulin 


Insulin is an anabolic, or building up, hormone. Of the numerous regula¬ 

tors of metabolism, insulin is the most important, targeting a number of 

organs and processes. One of its primary effects is blocking the breakdown 

of fat to fatty acids. Indirectly, insulin inhibits breakdown of fat and favors 

fat storage, as well as glucose storage in the form of glycogen. Uptake into 



An Introduction to Metabolism 



103 



muscle is also stimulated by recruitment of the glucose transporters, the 

GLUT4 receptors. GLUT4 is described as an “insulin-dependent recep¬ 

tor,” and clearance through this receptor was once considered to be the 

main effect of glucose, though it is now thought to be secondary. Overall, 

insulin clears glucose and stores it, catalyzes fat storage, and builds new 

proteins. Physiologically, it is a sign of good times. 


Looking ahead, in the case of diabetes, where there may be an absence of 

insulin (type 1) or poor response to circulating insulin (type 2), the effects 

on lipid metabolism might be more important than the effects on carbohy¬ 

drate metabolism, even though the primary problem rests with inadequate 

response to ingested glucose (contained in sugar or starch). The most imme¬ 

diate and salient feature of diabetes, however, is the hyperglycemia (high 

blood glucose) due to a failure to prevent hepatic production from the break¬ 

down of glycogen and the synthesis of new glucose via gluconeogenesis. The 

addition of dietary glucose on top of this will obviously make things worse. 



Ketone Bodies, Ketoacidosis, and Insulin 


Ketone bodies are acids, and as such, circulation must be protected against 

acidosis in their presence. Ketosis can become very high in type 1 diabetes, 

and ketoacidosis is one of the major threats of untreated type 1. So how is 

ketoacidosis prevented in people without diabetes and why does it occur in 

those with the disease? The idea is that, like most processes in metabolism, 

ketone-body synthesis and utilization is controlled by feedback loops. 

Here’s the process: 


1. When glucose is low, insulin is low and glucagon is high (in cases of 

starvation or low-carbohydrate diet). This leads to disinhibition of 

lipolysis in the fat cells, the process of fat breakdown that is normally 

repressed by insulin. The net effect is that fatty acid is increased and 

exported in the blood. Fatty acids in the blood, again, are referred to as 

free fatty acids (FFA) or nonesterified fatty acids (NEFA). 


2. The fatty acids are oxidized in the liver. Fatty acids are degraded by 

P-oxidation, which chops off two-carbon acetyl-CoA molecules stepwise. 


3. Acetyl-Co A is the substrate for energy metabolism in liver and other 

cells, but... 



104 



Nutrition in Crisis 



4. As acetyl-CoA increases, it is converted to the four-carbon ketone 

bodies (3-hydroxybutyrate and acetoacetate, and is transported to other 

tissues where it regenerates acetyl-Co A. As before, ketone bodies are a 

way of transporting acetyl-Co A. 


5. Ketone bodies regulate their own production in several ways. One way 

is to reduce the FFA that goes to the liver. 


6. There are two major feedback inhibitors: Back at the adipocyte (fat 

cell), the level of ketonemia (ketone bodies in the blood) is sensed by 

receptors. When the concentration is high, the ketone bodies turn off 

their own synthesis—that is, they inhibit lipolysis. In addition to this 

direct effect, ketone bodies also stimulate secretion of insulin from the 

pancreas. This, in turn, inhibits lipolysis. So you re left with lower fatty 

acids, lower acetyl-CoA in the liver, and lower ketone bodies. 


7. If glucose is still low, lipolysis is increased, and fatty acids “go back up.” 


8. Which process happens first? Well, it all happens at once. The whole 

system might not move because it is locked into the steady state of 

interlocking effects of glucose, insulin, ketone bodies, fatty acids, and 

everything else. The level of insulin biases the whole system in one 

direction or the other, but everything controls the final state. 


A person with type 1 diabetes, lacking insulin, cannot exert feedback 

control and is therefore in danger of ketoacidosis—since the ketone bodies 

are acids, high levels will increase acidity. 


An electronic amplifier provides a good analogy. The input from your 

sound system can be amplified greatly, in which case the amplifier is said 

to have very-high open-loop gain. Such a large signal, however, will also 

amplify the distortion: Small changes will cause large noise in the output. 

The distortion is dealt with by feeding some of the output back into the 

amplifier in the opposite direction of the incoming signal—so-called 

negative feedback. The gain (amplification) is thus greatly reduced, but the 

fidelity is increased. The process is similar in metabolism, where you don’t 

want big fluctuations. In type 1 diabetes, however, the fatty-acid signal 

cannot be adequately controlled. Its analogous to hooking your system up 

to an electric guitar whose signal cant be controlled and is said to drive 

your amplifier into saturation. You are left with an increase in distortion. 



-CHAPTER 6- 



Sugar, Fructose, and 

Fructophobia 



W hatever your degree of interest in nutrition, you are likely 

familiar with the media s crusade against sugar. There are now 

numerous scientific articles, picked up by the popular media, 

with titles like “Consuming Fructose-Sweetened, Not Glucose-Sweetened, 

Beverages Increases Visceral Adiposity and Lipids and Decreases Insulin 

Sensitivity in Overweight/Obese Humans.” 1 The sudden and pervasive 

spread of high-fructose corn syrup (HFCS) has made fructose a particular 

object of fear and loathing, despite the fact that HFCS has about the same 

proportions of fructose and glucose (55:45) as table sugar (50:50). The 

strange bedfellows in this political movement include Michael Bloomberg, 

the former mayor of New York, and Robert Lustig, a pediatric endocri¬ 

nologist. Exaggeration and alternative facts are the standard: Bloomberg 

claimed that banning large bottles of soda would solve the problem of 

obesity, and Lustig told us that sugar was as bad as alcohol or cocaine. How 

do we make sense of all this? 


We have always known that if you sit around all day eating candy, you 

will get fat. Conversely, cutting down on sugar, which is a carbohydrate, 

will contribute to weight loss and other benefits of a low-carbohydrate 

diet. The extent to which sugar, that is, sucrose, or its component fructose 

(sucrose is half glucose and half fructose), has a unique role in obesity and 

other effects of carbohydrate is not well understood. However the observed 

differences are not large, and as discussed below, on a low-carbohydrate 

diet, any fructose that is ingested will be converted to glucose. 


The problem with the numerous scientific papers claiming that fructose 

is dangerous is that they are all carried out against a background of 55 

percent total carbohydrate diets. If you are going to remove carbohydrates 



106 



Nutrition in Crisis 



from your diet, you might want to know whether fructose or glucose 

should be eliminated—that is, whether sugar is better, worse, or the same 

as starch. What these studies show you, instead, is how bad it is to add 

fructose on top of existing nutrients, or to replace glucose with fructose. 

It is not reasonable to think that we will solve things by keeping a high- 

carbohydrate diet and switching from orange juice to the whole-grain 

bread that always seems slightly indigestible (at least to some of us). 


The problem is that we dont really have an answer when it comes to 

fructose. For people with diabetes or metabolic syndrome, or even those 

who are overweight, the data are clear: Total carbohydrate restriction any 

carbohydrate—is the most effective therapy. Yet we don't know the extent to 

which removing fructose is a player in these therapeutic effects because of 

the insulin effect, if you have diabetes, it is almost always better to remove 

starch than to remove sugar. The sugars are interconvertible and effects differ 

depending on conditions. You dont need credentials to do science, but what 

professionals do know is that you cant simply make guesses. Because of the 

complexities—depending on conditions more than half of ingested fruc¬ 

tose is turned to glucose—you cant extrapolate from isolated experiments. 

Sometimes, you need the data. Biochemists are rare in nutrition because we 

simply dont know enough. I continue to admit that I am just about the only 

biochemist dumb enough to get involved here. 



In Defense of Sugar (and Fructose) 


Writing a “defense” of sugar seems very odd. In some ways, it’s in the same 

vein as the defenses of saturated fat that I have written in the past. In those 

cases, I stressed the need for perspective. People have been making and 

eating cheese for as long as they knew how. What am I defending? Eggs 

Benedict? Bearnaise sauce? Soppressata sausage? These have been part of 

our culture for generations. Were we really supposed to believe that some 

recent earth-shattering scientific discovery meant we’d been consuming 

poison all along? It didn’t seem to make sense, and in fact, the science was 

very poor, sometimes embarrassingly poor. It’s the same case with fructose. 

Sugar is a food, and while nobody denies that many in the population are 

plagued by overconsumption due to excessive availability, it is still a feature 

of la cuisine, haute and otherwise. Chocolate mousse is as much a part of 



Sugar, Fructose, and Fructophobia 



107 



our culture as Wagner. Some of us have to limit mousse to very small doses, 

but for many, that is also true of Wagner. 



The Threat of Fructophobia 


Rob Lustig is a nice guy. Everybody says this before criticizing his increas¬ 

ingly unrestrained crusade against sugar. He is almost as ubiquitous as 

HFCS itself. His YouTube and 60 Minutes performances, among others, 

have raised him to the standard of scientific spokesperson against fructose. 

One of Lustig’s YouTube videos begins with the question, “What do the 

Atkins diet and Japanese food have in common?” The answer is supposed 

to be the absence of sugar but, of course, that isn’t true. 


Sugar is an easy target. These days, if you say “sugar,” people think of oft- 

vilified foods such as Pop-Tarts or Twinkies, rather than red wine poached 

pears or tamagoyaki, the sweet omelet that is a staple in Bento Boxes. 

Here’s a question, though: If you look on the ingredients list for Pop-Tarts, 

what is the first ingredient, the one in largest amount? It s not sugar; it s 

enriched flour. When I posted this on my blog, one person claimed that 

although flour is the first ingredient, if you add up the high-fructose corn 

syrup, dextrose, and other types of sugar, their sum is larger. Some have 

indeed suggested that this is a strategy to hide total sugar content. It’s an 

interesting idea, but easily disproven: The label clearly says that there are 

38 grams of total carbohydrate and only 17 grams of sugar. 


Whether or not they think that carbohydrates are inherently fattening, 

most people agree that by focusing on fat, the nutritional establishment 

gave people license to overconsume carbohydrates, thus contributing to 

the obesity epidemic. Now, by focusing specifically on fructose, the AHA, 

USDA, and other organizations are giving implicit license to overconsume 

starch—its almost guaranteed since these agencies are still down on fat 

and protein. The additional threat is that in an environment of fructopho¬ 

bia, the only studies on fructose that will be funded are those with subjects 

consuming high levels of carbohydrate. In such studies the two sugars, 

glucose and fructose, interact metabolically and sometimes synergistically, 

so deleterious effects of fructose are likely to be found. The results will 

likely be generalized to all conditions, and as with lipophobia, there will be 

no null hypothesis. 



108 



Nutrition in Crisis 



The barrier to introducing some common sense into the discussion is, 

again, that we really don't have the answers. We don’t have enough data.The 

idea that fructose is a unique agent in increasing triglycerides (fat in the blood) 

is greatly exaggerated. High-carbohydrate diets lead to high triglycerides, 

and there are indeed conditions where sugar has a worse effect than starch, 

but the differences between the effects of fructose specifically and those of 

carbohydrates in general are small. The greatest threat of fructophobia is that 

we wont find out what the real effect of fructose is. Furthermore, the two 

regimes being compared in current studies are both high in carbohydrate, 

and because the absolute level of the triglycerides is high in both cases, it 

is likely that the biggest difference would be seen if either of the outcomes 

were compared to the results of a low-carbohydrate diet. 


The single most important question to ask with regard to sugar is this: 

For a person on the so-called standard, high-carbohydrate American diet, 

is it better to replace carbohydrate (any carbohydrate) with fat (any fat 

except trans-fat), or to replace fructose with glucose? Ignoring subtleties, 

to a first approximation, which is better? 


We might not know the answer to that question in every case, but the 

studies that have been done clearly indicate that replacing carbohydrate 

with fat is more beneficial. We also know that ethanol is not processed like 

fructose despite what many claim on the internet. Although the pathways 

converge, like almost all substrates that are used for energy at the entry to 

the TCA cycle, they are different. It's just not true that there is a similar¬ 

ity. We also know that although some claim glycogen is not formed from 

fructose, and Lustig shows a metabolic pathway from which glycogen is 

absent, fructose does actually give rise to glycogen. Under most condi¬ 

tions, fructose is a preferred substrate for glycogen synthesis. (In fact, for 

some period in the history of chemistry, fructose was primarily considered 

a “glycogenic substrate.”) Of course, fructose must first be converted to 

glucose. Claude Bernard knew that fructose could give rise to glycogen, 

but he couldn't understand why he wasn't able to find any fructose within 

the glycogen itself. 


To return to the comparison that is often made between sugar and 

ethanol, it is possible that sugar and ethanol have behavioral effects in 

common, but this is not due to similarities in metabolism. Moreover, the 

behavioral effects are not even settled within the psychology community: 



Sugar, Fructose, and Fructophobia 



109 



Alcoholism is far different from sugar addiction, if there is such a thing. 

While there is no definite agreement, addictive has formal definitions in 

behavioral psychology, and polishing off the whole bag of chocolate chip 

cookies might not technically qualify as addictive behavior. Some restraint 

is necessary. Alcohol-associated liver disease is a well-characterized, life- 

threatening condition, and many people do die from it. The idea that 

fructose can have the same effect, either physically or psychologically, has 

no basis in science and is deeply offensive to people who have had personal 

experience with alcohol-related illness and death. 


In the end, nobody has ever been admitted to a hospital for an overdose 

of fructose. People might say that your diabetes was caused by fructose 

consumption, but you cant be admitted to a hospital on somebody's opinion 

of what you did wrong. There is no diagnosis called “fructose poisoning.” If 

you are admitted to the hospital for type 2 diabetes, that is the diagnosis. 


The Threat of Policy 


The numerous scientific papers that find something wrong with fructose 

may have had little impact on people s behavior, but possibly for that 

reason, the fructophobes have taken new steps. Convinced of the correct¬ 

ness of their position, they have taken their case to politicians who are 

always eager to tax and regulate. There is an obvious sense of deja vu as 

another group of experts tries to use the American population as guinea 

pigs for a massive population experiment, along the lines of the low-fat 

fiasco under which we still suffer (not to mention the historical example of 

alcohol prohibition). It is not just that the lipophobe movement had unin¬ 

tended consequences (think margarine and trans-fats) but rather that, as 

numerous people have pointed out, the science was never there for low-fat 

to begin with. In other words, before science is turned into policy, we have 

to address the question of whether the science is any good to begin with. 



Fructose in Perspective 


Contrary to popular myth, the “Twinkie Defense” of Harvey Milk's 

murderer Dan White did not argue that the defendant was possessed by 

some kind of sugar rush. Rather, the defense claimed that he was propelled 



no 



Nutrition in Crisis 



to homicide by his depressed state, and that this deranged mental condi¬ 

tion was indicated by his consumption of junk food. (He had previously 

been a health nut.) It was a strange defense, because most of us think of 

depressives as going for suicide rather than homicide but in any case, 

sugar did not make him do it. 


Fructose is a normal nutrient and metabolite. It is a carbohydrate and 

it is metabolized in the metabolic pathways of carbohydrate processing. If 

nothing else, your body makes a certain amount of fructose. Fructose, not 

music (the food of love according to Shakespeare), is the preferred fuel of 

sperm cells. Fructose formed in the eye can be a risk but its cause is gener¬ 

ally very high glucose. The polyol reaction involves sequential conversion 

of glucose to sorbitol and then to fructose. 


One truly bizarre twist in the campaign against fructose was the study 

by Miguel Lanaspa and his colleagues 2 that attempted to show that the 

deleterious effects of glucose were due to its conversion to fructose via the 

polyol reaction. The paper was technical, but many people reading it in 

Nature don’t think that the conclusion really followed from the data—it 

would be a major change in metabolic thinking, and if it were true* it 

would have been accepted. If the idea has not actually been excluded, then 

it must be some expression of the need to say that, somehow or another, 

everything bad is caused by fructose. 


My review with Eugene Fine, ^Fructose in Perspective, was published 

in 2013 in Nutrition and Metabolism with the following conclusion: 


We all agree that reducing sugar intake as a way of reducing 

calories or limiting carbohydrates, is a good thing, especially 

for children, but it is important to remember that fructose 

and sucrose are carbohydrates. We know well the benefits of 

reducing total carbohydrate. How much of such benefits can be 

attributed to removing sugar is unknown.The major point is that 

these sugars are rapidly incorporated into normal carbohydrate 

metabolism. In some sense, any unique effects of sucrose that are 

observed are due to fructose acting as a kind of super-glucose. 3 


Again, the threat is thinking that fructose is sufficiently different from 

glucose that substituting glucose for fructose is guaranteed to be better. It’s 



Sugar, Fructose, and Fructophobia 



111 



The Evolutionary Argument 


It is still frequently said that our metabolism is not designed to 

handle the high input of fructose that was absent in our evolu¬ 

tion and that is present in our current environment. Although 

fructose might not have been widely available in Paleolithic 

times, that doesn’t mean that high consumption was uncom¬ 

mon. It is likely that, for our ancestors, finding the rare berry 

bush was like finding a coupon for Haagen-Dazs. Moderation 

was not the key word. While few would argue that wide avail¬ 

ability or high consumption is without risk, it is important to 

hew to the science. There is a big diffeience between saying that 

continued ingestion of high sugar (or high carbohydrates, or 

high anything) is not good, and saying that we do not have the 

metabolic machinery to deal with high intake, or that it is a 

foreign, toxic substance. 



not. Our review was a technical analysis of the literature, summarized in a 

rather complicated figure showing what is known of the two sugars. That 

figure might not be easily accessible to everybody, but figure 6.1, a simplified 

version of the one in the study, explains some of the key points. Fructose 

and glucose follow separate paths initially, but both six-carbon sugars are 

broken down to three-carbon fragments, the triose phosphates. These triose 

ph osphates are the intermediates in the effects of high carbohydrate inges¬ 

tion: high plasma triglycerides and low HDL (“good cholesterol”). 


The question for anybody who wants to attribute special properties to 

fructose is how an atom in a triose phosphate knows whether it came from 

fructose or from glucose. Of course, it doesn’t, and the outstanding feature 

of fructose metabolism is that it is part of glucose metabolism. Figure 6.1 

shows the path by which fructose can be turned into glucose, and under the 

appropriate conditions, into glycogen. As much as 60 percent of ingested 

fructose can be converted to glucose via gluconeogenesis. So, is there 



112 



Nutrition in Crisis 




// \ 


GLYCER0L-3-P PYRUVATE * T LACTATE 



TAG ETC TCA CYCLE 


Figure 6.1. A model of hepatic fructose metabolism. Key notes include: (1] Fructose 

stimulates glycogen storage; (2) fructose and glucose share common intermediates; 

(3) fructose can be converted to glucose; [4] ingested fructose calls for more glucose; 

and [5] generally, the liver expects glucose and fructose to come in together. 



really no difference between fructose and glucose? It turns out that the 

differences are small, and those that exist are due to kinetic (rate) effects 

rather than overall pattern of processing. Fructose is rapidly processed. 

Technically, fructokinase, the first enzyme to process fructose, has a low 

Km, which refers to the fact that it doesn’t take much fructose to get its 

metabolism going at a high rate. The carbons from fructose appear in the 

triose phosphates very quickly. So, if one wanted to make a grand state¬ 

ment, it might be that fructose is, as we called it, a kind of super-glucose. 


This is not an academic conclusion, however. We still need to know 

whether sugar acts primarily as a carbohydrate. Again, if we remove sugar 

and replace it with starch, how will the results compare with the many trials 

that show benefits of removing carbohydrate across the board and replac¬ 

ing it with fat? Nobody has directly made the comparison. The experiment 

has never been done. Experiments with real low-carbohydrate diets give 

far more dramatic results than those that replace fructose with glucose. 










Sugar, Fructose, and Fructophobia 



113 



One important phenomenon to note: The amount of glucose in the 

human diet is always greater than the amount of fructose (see figure 6.2). 

It is unlikely that anybody eats only sugar and no starch (which, again, is 

all glucose), and even HFCS has only slightly more fructose than glucose. 

Pure glucose is used for various effects in commercial food preparation. 

You will therefore almost never see pure fructose in the absence of glucose 

in the human diet. Moreover, ingested fructose triggers an increase in the 

glucokinase, the first glucose-processing enzyme in the liver, leading to 

further uptake of glucose. In other words, fructose calls for glucose—the 

liver expects the two sugars together. This is important, in that adminis¬ 

tering fructose alone is clearly detrimental. When it was discovered that 

fructose does not stimulate insulin secretion, some thought that fructose 

might be a good replacement for glucose for people with diabetes—again 

reflecting the idea that everybody needs sugar to be happy. It turned out 

that consuming fructose without glucose is dangerous. It is important to 

recognize, however, that the effects of fructose alone are not comparable to 

those of fructose in combination with glucose. 


In demonizing fructose across the board, we have to ask whether we 

are making the same rush to judgment that we did with fat. We said 

that dietary fat would give you heart disease, and a cascade of changes in 

medical practice flowed from that, but it has never been shown that fat 

gives you heart disease. A whole human population went all out to replace 

fat with carbohydrate, and this led to an increase in obesity and diabetes. 

Carbohydrate restriction remains the best strategy for obesity, diabetes, 

and metabolic syndrome. The specific contribution of removal of fructose 

or sucrose to this effect remains unknown. Unknown is the key word. 



Sweetener Consumption 


What about sweetener consumption? Surprisingly, it hasn’t gone up as 

much as you might have thought—about 15 percent, according to the 

USDA. One question is whether this increase is disproportionately due 

to fructose. The data show that, in fact, the ratio of fructose to glucose 

has remained constant over the last forty years. While sugar or HFCS is 

the main sweetener, pure glucose is sometimes used in the food industry 

and has remained at a constant 20 percent, explaining the deviation from 



Per Capita Availability (Ibs/person/year) Pounds per Year 



114 



Nutrition in Crisis 



100 



80 


Total HFCS and Sugar 




1970 1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 


Year 


140 




q-u 


1970 1975 1980 1985 1990 1995 2000 2005 


Year 


Figure 6.2. Sweetener consumption data, 1970-2005. Data from the USDA Sweetener 

Yearbook tables and the USDA Food Availability Data System. 


1:1, which would be expected (see figure 6.2). There is, however, more 

glucose than fructose in the food supply. One might argue that despite 

the constant ratio, the absolute increase in fructose has a more perni¬ 

cious effect than the increased glucose—but, of course, you would have 













Sugar, Fructose, and Fructophobia 



115 



to prove that. Figures 6.1 and 6.2 suggest that you would have to be 

careful in determining whether the effect of increased sweetener is due 

to fructose or to glucose, or whether it is the effect of one on the other, 

or the effect of insulin and other hormones on both. 



Scapegoats 


For a good example of the hyperbole surrounding discussion of fructose, 

consider an article in Mother Jones by Gary Taubes and Cristin Kearns 

Couzens called “Big Sugar’s Sweet Little Lies.” 4 The article is a fascinating, 

well-researched story of how the sugar industry has pushed its products, 

but the connection the authors make to the tobacco industry’s attempts 

to bury information and maintain profits is an overreach. I suspect one 

does not have to look hard for industries that try to sell their product 

by underhanded means, but the ethical problems in the case of tobacco 

were related to the product, not its promotion. Sugar does not functionally 

resemble tobacco in any way. The goal, of course, is to tax or otherwise 

impose punishment for sugar consumption—the first reaction of politi¬ 

cians is to punish and tax. The decline in cigarette smoking is cited as the 

effect of taxation. The question of whether reduction in cigarette consump¬ 

tion was a response to financial pressure or education is testable, however: 

If the former case is true, reduction in smoking should be greater in 

lower-income economic groups, and it should be opposite in the latter case 

(sources such as the Gallup polls show the latter case is true). The Mother 

Jones’s piece described a large rise in consumption of sugar sweeteners that 

was accompanied, in turn, by “a surge in the chronic diseases increasingly 

linked to sugar.” You can’t really expect a popular article to stand up to 

academic analysis, but the sentence is tricky—really illogical. It says that 

“the increase” in sugar is linked to diseases “linked to sugar.’’You can’t use 

“linked” twice. The link of chronic disease to sugar is exactly what’s in 

question. The graphics in the article tried to bring out the statistics, but 

the presentation is misleading. Mother Jones is not Annalen der Physik, of 

course, but some degree of precision is still required. In fact, the increase in 

the past thirty years is quite small. Using the numbers in the article: 



% Increase in sugar = 12 / 120 = 10% 



116 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Other data show that the increase was 15 percent, so we can even 

go with that number. For comparison, in a comparable time period, the 

increase in the United States for total carbohydrate was 23.4 percent for 

men and 38.4 percent for women. The idea, however, is that sugar has a 

very powerful, almost catalytic effect—in other words, a little increase in 

sugar is supposed to bring about big changes in health. So what happened 

in the thirty years that is presumed to have been a consequence of the 9 

percent increase in added sugars? Again, from the figure in the paper: 



% Increase in diabetes 

% Increase US children who are obese 

% Increase US adults who are obese 



4.3 



2.5 = 


172% 


11.4 



5.5 = 


207.2% 


20.7 



15 = 


138% 



Is fructose that powerful? If it is, the recommended reduction would 

certainly be a good thing, but how likely is it that fructose was truly the 

root of these increases, and if it is that powerful, could it ever be adequately 

reduced? A 9 percent or even 15 percent increase in sugar consumption is 

supposed to have caused a major increase in obesity and diabetes; the abso¬ 

lute changes are small, but still significant since these statistics represent 

the entire population. The experiment to test whether fructose is actually 

that powerful would be easy to conduct: Compare removal of fructose with 

removal of glucose and the results should be evident. Again, that experi¬ 

ment hasn’t been done. Why not? We have good experiments showing that 

if you take out carbohydrates and put in fat, you get significant benefit. If 

fructose is the most harmful of the carbohydrates, it should not be hard to 

show that it was fructose that was the controlling player in these compari¬ 

sons. It is surprising, and perhaps even suspicious, that the experiment 

hasn’t been done. 



-CHAPTER 7- 



Saturated Fat 


On Your Plate or in Your Blood? 



A high-fat diet in the presence of carbohydrate is different than 

a high-fat diet in the presence of low-carbohydrate. Failure to 

understand this principle, and the resulting failure to adequately 

test the effect exerted by carbohydrate, stands as a major source of confu¬ 

sion. Numerous reports in the medical and popular literature describe the 

effect of a “high-fat diet” or even “a single high-fat meal.”The source of the 

high fat, however, might be a slice of carrot cake, a Big Mac, or something 

else that is also very high in carbohydrate. 


On the subject of saturated fat, the studies from Jeff Volek’s laboratory, 

then at the University of Connecticut, provide the most telling evidence. 

Science does not run on majority rule. The total number of experiments 

is less important than the scientific design of the individual trials and 

whether it is easy to interpret their results. A study from Volek’s lab on 

forty volunteers with metabolic syndrome provides a classic case, carefully 

controlled and unambiguous. 


Particularly striking, the study found that when the blood of volun¬ 

teers was assayed for saturated fatty acids, those who had been on a 

low-carbohydrate diet had lower levels than those on an isocaloric 

low-fat diet—this despite the fact that the low-carbohydrate diet had 

three times the amount of dietary saturated fat as the low-fat diet. How 

was this possible? Well, that’s what metabolism does. For those on the 

low-carbohydrate diet, the saturated fat was oxidized, while the low- 

fat group was making new saturated fatty acid. Volek’s former student, 

Cassandra Forsythe, extended the idea by showing how, even under 

eucaloric conditions (no weight loss), dietary fat has relatively little 

impact on fat in the blood. 1 



118 



Nutrition in Crisis 



A barrier to understanding the role of saturated fat rests with the 

emphasis on “diets,” where it is impossible to come to an agreement on 

definitions and where an accidental or individual response might happen 

to work for an individual or small group of people (e.g., the generic ad hoc 

grapefruit diet). We would do better by speaking, instead, of basic prin¬ 

ciples. The key principle is that dietary carbohydrate, directly or indirectly, 

through insulin and other hormones, controls what happens to ingested 

and stored fatty acids. Carbohydrate has a catalytic effect—it controls what 

happens to other nutrients. The fat in the Big Mac will not constitute any 

risk if you chuck the bun. You are not what you eat. You are what you do 

with what you eat. 


The question of saturated fat is critical. The scientific evidence shows 

that dietary saturated fat, in general, has no effect on cardiovascular disease, 

obesity, or probably anything else—but plasma saturated fatty acids do. In 

particular, plasma saturated fatty acids can provide a cellular signal, and they 

exacerbate insulin resistance. If you study dietary saturated fatty acids under 

conditions where carbohydrate is high, or, more importantly, if your study 

effects are in rodents, where plasma fat better correlates with dietary fat, 

then you will confuse plasma fat with dietary fat. There is a real difference. 


To understand the problem, recall that strictly speaking, there are only 

saturated fatty acids (SFAs). What is called “saturated fat” simply means 

those fats that have a high percentage of SFAs. Things that we identify as 

“saturated fats,” such as butter, usually contain only 50 percent saturated 

fatty acids. Coconut oil is probably the only fat that is almost entirely 

saturated fatty acids, but because those acids are medium-chain length, 

they are usually considered a special case. 


In Volek’s experiment, forty overweight subjects were randomly assigned 

to one of two diets 2 : a very low-carbohydrate ketogenic diet (VLCKD), 

which provided a macronutrient distribution of about 12 percent carbo¬ 

hydrate, 59 percent fat, and 28 percent protein; or a low-fat diet (LFD) 

composed of 56 percent carbohydrate, 24 percent fat, and 20 percent 

protein. The group was unusual in that they were all overweight and would 

all be characterized as having metabolic syndrome. All subjects demon¬ 

strated the features of atherogenic dyslipidemia, a subset of metabolic 

syndrome markers that describes a poor lipid profile: high triacylglycerol 

(TAG), low HDL-C, and high small-dense LDL (so-called pattern B). 



Saturated Fat 



119 



What’s striking in Volek’s work is the difference in weight loss between 

the two diet regimens. Participants in the study were not specifically coun¬ 

seled to reduce calories but both groups spontaneously reduced caloric 

intake. (Apparently, people in diet studies tend to automatically reduce 

calories.) However the level of weight loss between the two groups, due 

to the macronutrient composition of the plans, was dramatically different. 

People on the VLCKD lost twice as much weight on average as those on 

the LFD despite the similar caloric intake. Although there was substantial 

individual variation (see figure 9.2 on page 134), nine of twenty subjects 

in the VLCKD group lost 10 percent of their starting body weight, more 

weight than was lost by any of the subjects in the LFD group. In fact, 

nobody following the LFD lost as much weight as the average for the low- 

carbohydrate group. The major differences between the VLCKD and LFD 

groups appeared in the changes in whole body fat mass: 5.7 kilograms and 

3.7 kilograms, respectively. 


It is generally believed that deposition of fat in the abdominal region 

is more undesirable than subcutaneous fat. Abdominal fat was found to 

be reduced more in subjects on the VLCKD than in subjects following 

the LFD (-828 grams versus -506 grams). Volek’s study thus provides 

one of the more dramatic demonstrations of the benefits of carbohydrate 

restriction for weight loss. Similar results had preceded it, though. Those 

preceding studies had frequently been disparaged for increasing the 

amount of saturated fat as a sort of automatic criticism (whether or not 

any particular study actually increased saturated fat). Although the original 

“concern”was increased plasma cholesterol, eventually saturated fat became 

a generalized villain, and insofar as the science was concerned, the effects 

of plasma saturated fat were assumed to be due to dietary saturated fat. 

The surprising outcome of Volek’s study was that there was, in fact, inverse 

correlation between dietary and plasma SFA. It was surprising because 

the effect was so clear-cut that no statistics were needed, and because an 

underlying mechanism could explain the results. 



On Your Plate or in Your Blood? 


In Volek’s study the dietary intake of saturated fat for the people on the 

VLCKD averaged out to 36 grams per day, threefold higher than that of 



120 



Nutrition in Crisis 



the people on the LFD (12 grams per day). When the relative proportions 

of circulating SFAs in the triglyceride and cholesterol ester fractions were 

determined, however, they were actually lower in the low-carbohydrate 

group. Seventeen of twenty subjects on the VLCKD showed a decrease 

in total saturates, while the other three already had low values at baseline. 

In distinction, only half of the subjects consuming the LFD showed a 

decrease in SFA. When the absolute fasting TAG levels are taken into 

account (low-carbohydrate diets reliably reduce TAG), the absolute 

concentration of total saturates in plasma TAG was reduced by 57 percent 

in the low-carbohydrate group compared to only 24 percent reduction 

in the low-fat group—again, this is despite the fact that LFD group had 

reduced their dietary saturated fat intake. How could this happen? The 

low-fat group reduced their SFA intake by one-third, yet had more SFA in 

their blood than the low-carbohydrate group who had actually increased 

intake. Metabolism is about change. Chemistry is about transformation. 



De Novo Lipogenesis 


To review the major features of metabolism bearing on fat, there are 

roughly two kinds of fuel: glucose and acetyl-CoA. The big principle is 

that you can make acetyl-CoA from glucose, but in most cases you cant 

make glucose from acetyl-Co A—or, more generally, you can make fat 

from glucose but you cant make glucose from fat. So, how do you make 

fat from glucose? Part of the picture is the process of making new fatty 

acids, known as de novo lipogenesis (DNL), or more accurately de novo 

fatty acid synthesis. The mechanism for making new fatty acids is, in a 

rough sort of way, the reverse of breaking them down. You successively 

patch together two-carbon acetyl-Co A units until you reach the chain 

length of sixteen carbons: palmitic acid, the saturated fatty acid that was 

found in higher concentrations in the subjects on low-fat diets in Volek’s 

experiment. Palmitic acid can then be elongated to stearic acid (18:0) or 

desaturated to the unsaturated fatty acid, palmitoleic acid (16:l-n7, 16 

carbons, one unsaturation at carbon 7). 


The critical part is getting the process going. The first step is the formation 

of a three-carbon compound called malonyl-CoA, a process under the control 

of insulin. Malonyl-CoA enters into the synthetic process and simultaneously 



Saturated Fat 



121 



prevents transport of fatty acid into the mitochondrion where it would be 

oxidized. If you are making new fatty acid, you don’t want to burn it. New 

fatty acid is a reasonable explanation for the increased SFA in the low-fat 

group. The low-carbohydrate group, on the other hand, would be expected to 

have higher insulin levels on average, encouraging diversion of calories into 

fatty acid synthesis and repressing oxidation. How could this be tested? 


It turns out that the unsaturated fatty acid, palmitoleic acid (16:l-n7), is 

not common in the diet and therefore stands as a good indicator of synthesis. 

The same enzyme that catalyzes conversion of palmitic acid also catalyzes 

conversion of stearic acid (18:0) to the unsaturated fatty acid oleic acid 

(18:ln-7), as in olive oil. The enzyme in question is named for the second 

reaction, stearoyl-CoA desaturase-1 (SCD-1). SCD-1 is membrane-bound, 

which means that it is not swimming around the cell looking for fatty acids, 

but instead is closely tied to DNL (waiting at the end of the assembly line 

so to speak), and preferentially desatufates newly formed fatty acids: palmitic 

acid to palmitoleic acid and stearic to oleic. It is unsurprising, then, that the 

data from Volek’s experiment show a 31 percent decrease in palmitoleic acid 

(16:ln-7) in the blood of subjects on the low-carbohydrate group with litde 

overall change in the average response in the low-fat group. The low-fat group 

was making saturated fatty acid more than the low-carbohydrate group. 



Saturated Fat in Your Blood 


Cassandra Forsythe, Volek’s student at the time, extended this work to 

an experiment on weight maintenance. It is commonly claimed that 

the physiologic effects of low-carbohydrate diets are linked to weight 

loss rather than to an inherent response to reduction in carbohydrate, 

so it is important to tackle this objection. In her experiment, men were 

assigned to one of two different weight-maintaining diets, both low in 

carbohydrate, for six weeks. The first of the diets was designed to be high 

in SFA (dairy fat and eggs), and the other was designed to be higher in 

unsaturated fat from both polyunsaturated (PUFA) and monounsaturated 

(MUFA) fatty acids (fish, nuts, omega-3 enriched eggs, and olive oil). For 

the SFA-carbohydrate-restricted diet, the relative percentages of SFA, 

MUFA, and PUFA were 31,21, and 5, respectively. For the UFA diet, the 

percentages were 17,25, and 15.The results as stated in Forsythes study: 



122 



Nutrition in Crisis 



The most striking finding was the lack of association between 

dietary SFA intake and plasma SFA concentrations. Compared 

to baseline, a doubling of saturated fat intake on the CRD-SFA 

(carbohydrate-restricted diet with high saturated fatty acid) did 

not increase plasma SFA in any of the lipid fractions, and when 

saturated fat was only moderately increased on the CRD-UFA, 

the proportion of SFA in plasma TAG was reduced from 31.06% 

to 27.48 mol%. Since plasma TAG was also reduced, the total 

SFA concentration in plasma TAG was decreased by 47% after 

the CRD-UFA, similar to the 57% decrease we observed in over¬ 

weight men and women after 12 week of a hypocaloric CRD. 3 


The bottom line: Dietary carbohydrate, rather than dietary SFA, controls 

plasma SFA. Therefore, while it is widely held that the type of fat is more 

important than the amount, this is not a universal principle, and it becomes 

less important if carbohydrates are low. But what about the amount? A 

widely cited paper by Raatz et al. 4 suggested, as indicated by the title, that 

“Total Fat Intake Modifies Plasma Fatty Acid Composition in Humans.” 

The data in the paper, however, show that the differences between high-fat 

and low-fat were, in fact, minimal. How can you say one thing when your 

data show something else? One doesn’t know what was on the authors 

minds, and maybe they interpreted things differently, but in general it seems 

that the literature takes an approach similar to that of lawyers: If the jury 

buys it, it doesnt matter whether or not its true. In scientific publishing, the 

jurors are the reviewers and the editors. If they are already convinced of the 

conclusion, if there is no voir dire, you will surely win the case. 


The bottom line is that distribution of types of fatty acid in plasma 

is more dependent on the level of dietary carbohydrate consumption 

than the level or type of dietary fat consumption. The work of Volek and 

Forsythe provides a good reason to focus on the carbohydrate content of 

your diet. What about the type of carbohydrate, though? In other words, is 

glycemic index important? Is fructose as bad as they say? Consistent with 

the small perturbation caused by fructose compared to glucose, as shown 

in the previous chapter, we have a good general principle: No change in the 

type of macro nutrient—carbohydrate or fat—will ever have the same kind 

of effect as replacing carbohydrate across the board with fat. 



-CHAPTER 8- 



Hunger 


What It Is, What to Do About It 



T he reporter from Mens Health asked me: “You finish dinner, even 

a satisfying low-carb dinner”—he is a low-carbohydrate person 

himself—“you are sure you ate enough but you are still hungry. 

What do you do?” I gave him good advice: “Think of a perfectly broiled 

steak or steamed lobster with butter—some high protein, relatively high- 

fat meal that you usually like. If that doesn’t sound good, you are not 

hungry. You might want to keep eating. You might want something sweet. 

You might want to feel something rolling around in your mouth, but you 

are not hungry. Find something else to do—push-ups are good. If the steak 

does sound good, you might want to eat. Practically speaking, you might 

want to keep hard boiled eggs, kielbasa, something filling, around (and, 

of course, you don’t want cookies in the house).” I think this was good 

practical advice. My recommendation was based on the satiating effects of 

protein food sources, or perhaps the nonsatiating, or binge-inducing effect 

of carbohydrate. All this raises a larger question, though: What is hunger? 


We grow up thinking that hunger is our body’s way of telling us that we 

need food, but for most of us that is not the case. Few of us are so fit, or have 

so little body fat, or are so active, that our bodies start calling for energy if 

we miss lunch. Conversely, those of us who really like food generally hold 

to the philosophy that “any fool can eat when they’re hungry.” Passing up 

a really good chocolate mousse just because you are not hungry is like ... 

well, I don’t know what it’s like. Of course, if you are on a low-carbohydrate 

diet, you might pass it up for dietary reasons, or at least restrain yourself 

from eating too much. 


Getting to the point here, if I presented you with a multiple-choice 

question that asked what hunger is, the answer would be “all of the above.” 



124 



Nutrition in Crisis 



We feel hunger when we haven’t eaten for a while; we feel hunger if the 

food looks good; we feel hunger if we are in a social situation in which 

eating is going on (the spread of petits fours that were in the lobby at the 

break in an obesity conference, the congressional prayer breakfast, or the 

Pavlovian lunch bell). 


Or we might eat because we think it is time to eat. This point was made 

by the Restoration poet and rake, John Wilmot, Earl of Rochester. Wilmot 

is famous for his bawdy poetry, raunchy even by today’s standards, but his 

“Satyr against Reason and Mankind,” which is more commonly included 

in texts on eighteenth-century literature, makes fun of dumb rules and 

phony reason: 


My reason is my friend, yours is a cheat; 


Hunger calls out, my reason bids me eat; 


Perversely, yours your appetite does mock: 


This asks for food, that answers, “Whats o’clock?” 


This plain distinction, sir, your doubt secures: 


Tis not true reason I despise, but yours. 


Americans have not conquered this problem, and we may in fact have 

made it worse. A diet experiment invariably includes a snack as if it has 

the same standing as breakfast, lunch, and dinner (anecdotally, the number 

of people who prefer to go without breakfast suggests that that meal is at 

least not for everyone). Visitors remark on how Americans are eating all 

the time, not just at meals. If you maintain such a habit, it doesn’t take long 

until you are hungry all the time. 


Different people have different responses to external cues. In experi¬ 

ments in which subjects are interrogated, but incidentally have snacks 

available—a bowl of crackers on the table, for example—it is not surprising 

that thin people regulate their intake by the clock on the wall. Overweight 

people, in distinction, are less sensitive to the clock and dip into the snacks 

even if “it’s almost dinner time.” Similarly, at Union Theological Seminary 

in New York, the school for training rabbis, it is the overweight students 

who adhere better to fasting on high holy days. Consumption is less 

connected to internal (physiologic) cues and external (religious) reasons 

can have control. 



Hunger 



125 



The psychologist B. F. Skinner 1 described the problem in a characteristi¬ 

cally dense way: 


“I am hungry” may be equivalent to “I have hunger pangs,” 

and if the verbal community had some means of observing the 

contractions of the stomach associated with pangs, it could pin 

the response to these stimuli alone. It may also be equivalent to 

“I am eating actively.” A person who observes that he is eating 

voraciously may say, “I really am hungry,” or, in retrospect, “I was 

hungrier than I thought,” dismissing other evidence as unreli¬ 

able. “I am hungry” may also be equivalent to “It has been a long 

time since I have had anything to eat,” although the expression 

is most likely to be used in describing future behavior: “If I miss 

my dinner, I shall be hungry.” 


What Skinners saying is that whatever the actual causes of eating 

behavior, the behavior itself may precede the description of the “motivation 

to eat.” In other words, we tend to identify a feeling that is associated with 

eating behavior as the cause of the behavior. “I am hungry” may also be 

equivalent to “I feel like eating” in the sense of “I have felt this way before 

when I have started to eat.”The point is that “hungry” only means you are 

in a situation where you are used to eating. It doesn't mean that feeling 

hungry will make you eat, or, more importantly, that you have to eat. 



Lessons from Vagotomy 


The vagus nerve contains many nerve fibers that facilitate communication 

between the brain and other parts of the body (a nerve is a collection of 

nerve cells or neurons whose long extensions or axons are referred to as 

fibers). Cells that send signals from the brain to distant organs are called 

efferent. Efferent fibers in the vagus nerve regulate the digestive tract in 

various ways: enlargement of the stomach, for example, or secretions from 

the pancreas to deal with larger volumes of food (known to doctors as 

accommodation). Most of the fibers in the vagus nerve are sensory affer- 

ents (afferents carry information from the body to the brain) providing 

sensations of satiety and hunger as well as a feeling of discomfort when 



126 



Nutrition in Crisis 



we are full. (Efferent is usually pronounced “EE-ferent” to distinguish 

from afferents.) 


Vagotomy, cutting the vagus nerve, was historically practiced as a means 

of controlling ulcers, and is still a target, at least experimentally, for treating 

obesity. Dr. John Krai at the Department of Surgery at Downstate, who 

has performed such operations, described to me how patients complained 

that they had lost their appetite. He had to explain to them that you do not 

have to eat all the time, and that nothing bad will happen if you miss a few 

meals. Hunger is a signal that you are used to eating in a particular time or 

situation. You are not required to answer that signal. 


“You Eat Because You Are Fat” 


In trying to go beyond energy balance there is a tendency to think of 

hunger in terms of hormones, emphasizing regulation by the hypothala¬ 

mus, analogous to temperature regulation. The hormones in question are 

referred to as either orexigenic, increasing appetite (from the Greek: the 

Greek equivalent of bon appetite is kali orexi ), or anorexigenic, depressing 

appetite. While this is part of the picture, it leads to some confusion because 

the endocrine approach emphasizes hormonal output from the fat cell, and 

in some sense bypasses the question of how the fat cell got fat in the first 

place—that is, how it bypasses metabolism. More importantly, for animals 

and humans outside of a laboratory setup, behavior overrides hormones. 

The analogy is not entirely accurate in that animals (and humans) regulate 

their temperature hormonally only to a small extent. The major control of 

temperature is behavioral: We put on clothes and we hide in caves. 


An important aspect of this problem is the need to understand the error 

in “a calorie is a calorie.” One critique of the energy balance model runs 

something like this: 


dietary (other increased greater 


, , : -> insulin x 


carbohydrate hormones) appetite consumption 


In the extreme case, the explanation might boil down to: “You don’t get 

fat because you eat; you eat because you got fat.”This doesn’t make much 

sense. It sounds like one of the seemingly profound academic aphorism 



Hunger 



127 



that Woody Allen was so good at parodying: “All of literature is just a 

footnote to Faust.” I understand that it implies that the hormonal secre¬ 

tion from adipose tissue encourages eating—but again, it does not tell you 

why you got fat in the first place. It mixes up metabolism with behavior 

and implicitly accepts the idea that calories are what count—that it is the 

total energy you consume that matters, rather than how that energy is 

processed. Although macronutrients clearly differ in satiety, regardless of 

your hormonal state, if there is no food, you will not increase consumption. 

Also, the effects of insulin are not so clear-cut. Whereas metabolically 

insulin is anabolic, at the level of behavior it is probably anorexigenic in 

most cases. 


Why do we get fat? We get fat because we eat too much of the stuff that 

encourages excessive weight gain. We don’t know what that stuff is, but 

we know that it is not fat per se. Given the unambiguous effectiveness of 

carbohydrate restriction in reducing excess weight, it would be surprising 

if carbohydrate weren’t a big part of the picture. The so-called metabolic 

advantage (less weight gain per calorie), where it exists, is a metabolic 

effect. The most likely mechanism appears to be due to the effect of insulin 

on rates of reaction: Anabolic (storage) steps might increase accumulation 

before competing feedback (breakdown) can catch up. As I will explain in 

chapter 9, it rests on nonequilibrium thermodynamics, 2 which recognizes 

the importance of rates as well as energy. 



What Can You Do About It? 


The suggestion at the beginning of this section was to make sure you know 

what kind of hunger you are talking about. Behavioral psychology stresses 

the difference between “tastes good,” and the technical term, “reinforcing,” 

which means that the food increases the probability that you will keep 

eating. Anecdotally, we all have the experience of saying, “I don’t know why 

I ate that. It wasn’t very good.” 


However little you eat to answer feelings of hunger, it is certainly bad 

advice to eat if you are not hungry Professional nutritionists, even the 

Atkins website, are always telling you to have a good breakfast. Why you 

would want to have any “good” meal if you are trying to lose weight is not 

easy to answer. The nutritionists claim that without breakfast, you will eat 



128 



Nutrition in Crisis 



too much at the next meal, as if in the morning you can make the rational 

decision to eat breakfast despite no desire for food, while at noon you are 

suddenly under the inexorable influence of urges beyond your control. A 

more reasonable way to put it would be: “If you find that you eat too much 

at lunch when you don't eat breakfast, then ... ,”but that is not the style of 

traditional nutrition. 



More on Behavior 


The principle in behavioral psychology is that how soon a behavior is 

reinforced is more important than the quality of the reinforcer. It is 

important to reemphasize that reinforcement is not always equivalent 

to reward. Reinforcement simply means increasing the likelihood that a 

behavior will reappear, even if that behavior doesn't feel totally good Junk 

food may be reinforcing in that it will make you eat more even though 

you realize afterward it didn't taste that good. Taste and mouth feel are so 

immediately reinforcing that, in all likelihood, only aversive stimulation 

can work well to discourage eating. There are positive ways to look at 

hunger. For example: Feeling hungry may mean that your diet is working, 

that you really are losing weight, and therefore you might stop eating 

before satiation. However encouraging that notion might be, it usually 

cant compete well with even the smell of food. You need something 

strongly negative. 


One of the more effective regimens is a diet strategy from Dr. Allen Fay, 

a psychiatrist in New York. It works like this: 


1. You pick an amount of weight you want to lose in the next week; you 

can pick zero but, of course, you cant go up. 


2. You write a check for $2,000 to an organization that you don't like 

(Republican National Committee, in my case) and give it to Dr. Fay. 

(Note: This is a personal choice. This is not a political book.) 


3. If, at the end of the week, you haven't hit the weight target, he mails 

the check. 


In some cases, Dr. Fay said, you don't even need money. One patient 

wrote a letter, poised to be sent to a right-to-life organization, telling 



Hunger 



129 



Exercise 


Hie only thing that people in nutrition agree on is the value of 

exercise. While it is not as important as diet for weight loss, it 

does positively interact with diet and has obvious benefit. One 

question that comes up is when to have meals in relation to 

exercise. Although outside my area of expertise, and likely to be 

an individual thing, there is some good guidance in the follow¬ 

ing old joke: 


The couple go to the doctor and don't want to have 

any more children but they don't want any artificial 

methods of birth control. The doctor recommends 

exercise. 



Husband: Before or after? 

Doctor: Instead of. 



them what a great job they were doing. The thought that she would get 

on their mailing list as a supporter was sufficiently unpleasant to keep 

her on target. 


You pick your own threat, of course—it is not a political method. Dr. 

Fay suggested the American Nazi Party, but I thought that they would only 

buy those shabby uniforms, whereas from my perspective, the Republicans 

would do real damage. My relation to Dr. Fay is partly professional 

(although he admits there are people who are beyond psychotherapy) and 

partly friend. For the technique to work, you must be distant enough from 

the person holding the check so that they will actually mail it, but close 

enough that you will not consider physical violence if they do. 


Imagery can help, but only up to a point. A major problem situation 

for dieters is that they have eaten what they want and feel satisfied, but 



130 



Nutrition in Crisis 



there is still food on their plate, which they pick at until they feel sick. 

My approach when I felt full was to imagine spiders coming out of the 

food. This worked for a while, but over time I noticed that I was losing my 

distaste of spiders. Eating has a very strong Pavlovian component. It is not 

nice to fool Mother Nature. 



-CHAPTER 9- 



Beyond 


“A Calorie Is a Calorie” 


An Introduction to Thermodynamics 



A rnold Sommerfeld was one of the great physicists in the develop¬ 

ment of quantum mechanics (theory of atomic structure). He was 

also generally considered to be an expert on most areas of physics. 

His take on thermodynamics: 



The first time I studied it, I thought that I understood it 

except for a few minor details. 


The second time I studied it, I thought that I didn’t under¬ 

stand it except for a few minor details. 


The third time I studied it, I knew I didnt understand it but it 

didn’t matter because I already knew how to use it. 1 



Can you really lose more weight on one diet than another if you 

consume the same number of calories? The question usually comes in 

response to a low-carbohydrate diet, where the so-called metabolic 

advantage promises you that cutting out carbohydrate will lead to 

reduced efficiency in storing fat. Folks go crazy when you suggest its 

true. Whenever a scientific paper presents data showing that such a thing 

really happens—that one'diet, usually low-carbohydrate, is more effec¬ 

tive than another—somebody always jumps in to say that it is impossible, 

and that it would violate the laws of thermodynamics. Like the cartoon 

characters who run over the cliff, remain suspended in thin air, and fall 

only when they realize that they aren’t on solid ground, somehow the data 

are expected to go away once thermodynamics is invoked. Of course, the 



132 



Nutrition in Crisis 



data can't violate the laws of thermodynamics. The real possibility is that 

the data are accurate and that the critic doesn't get it. Thermodynamics— 

the physics of heat, work, and energy—is a tough subject, and it takes real 

chutzpah to jump in where many physicists fear to tread. 


Although it may be difficult to grasp, thermodynamics is interesting. It 

has been described as the first revolutionary science. You probably don't 

really need it to study nutrition, but if you catch on to the basics, you will 

understand something that seems counterintuitive to many people. It will 

explain how you get more bang for your nutritional buck—that is, how you 

lose more weight per calorie. 


When you consider that the fundamental unit in nutrition is the calorie, 

a unit of energy, it seems likely that it would be worth knowing something 

about the physics of energy exchange. 



The Bottom Line on Metabolic Advantage 


Here are the four main principles of metabolic advantage that will guide our 


discussion. The rest of the chapter will explain and justify these conclusions: 


1., Metabolic advantage, or a better term, energy efficiency, is not contra¬ 

dicted by any physical law.Thermodynamics, in fact, more or less predicts 

variable energy efficiency. The way it has been discussed in nutrition is 

incorrect and does not conform to the way chemical thermodynamics is 

normally used. 


2. Arguments against metabolic advantage often rely on practical consider¬ 

ations: how small the effect is. Yet the same critics espouse the value of 

cumulative small effects, operative in diets where you explicitly control 

calories, where 50 calories a day is supposed to add up over a year—and 

it doesn't. Metabolism doesn’t work like that. Homeostatic (stabilizing) 

mechanisms compensate for simple changes in calories unless they are the 

right kind of calories, and in fact, the effects of different macronutrients 

can be dramatic. In any case, if there really is any change at all, that should 

be a call to figure out how to maximize that change, rather than ignore it. 


3. Even if you aren’t sure that metabolic advantage has been effectively 

demonstrated, it makes sense to try to make it work for you, since there 

is a great deal of potential scientific payoff. 



Beyond “A Calorie Is a Calorie 



133 



4. Several mechanisms, particularly substrate cycling and gluconeogenesis, 

are involved. Experimentally, inefficiencies in digestion and metabolic 

processing (the so-called thermic effect of feeding) contribute as well. 


Many people find metabolic advantage counterintuitive because the 

idea of energy conservation has been so deeply ingrained in their minds, 

I will explain the fallacy. I will present some of the data and then explain 

how the process plays out in terms of biochemical mechanisms. 


The Data 


There are basically two kinds of diet experiments. Some clearly show 

energy balance and some clearly do not. Here's an example of the former: 

If you take a normal person, keep them in a hospital room, and feed them 

constant calories (figure 9.1), you will find that it doesn't matter much how 

the calories are distributed among different foods. “Wide variations in the 

ratio of carbohydrate to fat do not alter total 24-h energy need” 2 —their 

weight will stay roughly constant. In some experimental cases, like this, a 

calorie is indeed just a calorie. Two people who are roughly similar in age 

and health will respond similarly to two isocaloric (same caloric value) 

diets regardless of the diet composition (amount of fat, carbohydrate, and 

protein).This means that, yes, calories count. Yet there are many exceptions. 


The energy balance shown in figure 9.1 is achieved by biologic mecha¬ 

nisms, not the laws of thermodynamics. In those cases, where everything 

balances out, it isn't physical laws but rather the unique characteristics of 



Days 


10 24 38 52 66 80 94 55 




51 



< -► 10% Fat--70% Fat-► 


Figure 9.1. A thirteen-week study of a subject first on 10 percent (75 percent carbo¬ 

hydrate) of energy intake as fat and then on 70 percent (15 percent carbohydrate) 

of energy intake as fat. From J. Hirsch et al., “Diet Composition and Energy Balance in 

Humans,” American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 67, no. 3 (1998): 551-55S. 






134 



Nutrition in Crisis 




Figure 9.2. Comparison of low-carbohydrate ketogenic diet and low-fat diet. From 

J. S. Volek et al., "Carbohydrate Restriction Has a More Favorable Impact on the Metabolic 

Syndrome than a Low Fat Diet" Lipids 44, no. 4 [2009): 297-309. 







Beyond “A Calorie Is a Calorie” 



135 



living systems that keep things constant through the process of homeosta¬ 

sis. Big rule: In biology almost everything is connected in feedback, and 

homeostatic mechanisms compensate for chemical changes. If you reduce 

your dietary intake of cholesterol, for example, your body will compensate 

by synthesizing new cholesterol. In view of this, the question we should 

be asking is, “How is energy balance possible when it is not predicted 

by thermodynamics?”; not, “How could there be different weight gain or 

loss for the same number of calories?” So let s look at the exceptions—the 

cases where the energy does not balance out—the second type of dietary 

experiment seen in the literature. 


The exceptions can be dramatic. The experiment from the laboratory 

of Jeff Volek, then at the University of Connecticut, described previously 

in chapter 7, studied forty overweight men and women with metabolic 

syndrome (high triglycerides, low HDL, or at least two other factors).They 

were assigned to one of two ad libitum diets: a very low-carbohydrate keto- 

genic diet (VLCKD) (% carbohydrate [CHO]:fat:protein = 12:59:28), or a 

low-fat diet (%CHO:fat:protein = 56:24:20.) The experiment lasted twelve 

weeks. Although neither group was specifically told to reduce calories, both 

groups did show a spontaneous decrease in energy intake—it seems that if 

you sign up for a diet experiment, you automatically eat less. 


Figure 9.2 shows the dramatic difference in performance between the 

VLCKD and the LFD. Figure 9.2 A indicates the average effect: Weight loss 

in the VLCKD group was dramatically better. In reading the medical litera¬ 

ture, however, it is important to ask about individual performance. People are 

different. Nobody loses an average amount of weight. People in both groups 

lost weight, but what is remarkable is the number of people on the VLCKD 

that lost a lot of weight (Figure 9.2J5). Half of the people on the VLCKD lost 

more weight than the single most successful subject on the LFD. 



Can You Trust Dietary Records? 


Once again, there is the problem of patient dietary records. These records 

are known to be inaccurate, but not wildly so. Values can be off by 20 

percent, but rarely by 50 percent, and are unlikely to be sole cause of the 

differences. Ketone bodies were measured, as well, so at least the VLCKD 

group did what they were told. It is important to reiterate that if the 



136 



Nutrition in Crisis 



differences were due to inaccurate reporting of dietary intake (that is, if 

the diets were truly different in caloric intake), the people on the VLCKD 

must have overreported what they ate and the people on LFD must have 

underreported what they ate, or both. Bottom line: Food records have very 

high error rates. They are meaningful if the differences in the observed 

experimental differences are large, or if there are indicators of compliance 

such as the presence of ketone bodies. 


Low-carbohydrate diets almost always win in a face-off with low-fat 

diets. Establishment nutritionists will write off the ties as wns, but they 

can only do so for so long. So if it is not just about the calories, where 

does thermodynamics really fit in? With the disclaimer of Sommerfeld's 

comments at the top of this chapter, I will give you a rough idea about how 

it's done in real biochemistry. 


Thermodynamics 


Thermodynamics is the physics of heat, work, and energy. The subject is 

simultaneously theoretical and mathematic, and fundamentally down to 

earth. Its roots are in the attempt to find out just how efficient a steam engine 

you can build. Thermodynamics came about during the industrial revolution, 

when the efficiency of steam engines was a big deal. In weight loss, we are 

really asking how efficiently food is utilized—much like the efficiency of a 

steam engine's fuel. The difficult side of thermodynamics is that its methods 

are highly mathematical and arcane, even for scientists. Thermodynamics 

has been described as “the science of partial differential equations,” which is 

not to everybody's taste. The results, however, give you very simple equations 

for predicting things. Its a combination of heavy-duty theory and practical 

application. This is what those of us who are interested in thermodynamics 

actually like about it, and its also the main theme of this book: Science with 

direct applicability. You get an equation that tells you whether you have a 

good steam engine, or, in fact, whether your food is fattening. 



Real Thermodynamics 


When people evoke the laws of thermodynamics, they're usually just talk¬ 

ing about the first law, the law of conservation of energy. Unfortunately, 



Beyond “A Calorie Is a Calorie 1 



137 



"conservation of energy” has become a sound bite, at the level of "Einstein 

said that everything is relative.” You have to know exactly what is being 

conserved, and more important, in thermodynamics, what is not. What 

follows is how thermodynamics is taught and used in science. The math 

is simple, but understanding depends on precise definitions and careful 

logic. What I’m about to cover many not be necessary for nutrition, but 

since thermodynamics is so often invoked, it is worth going through at 

least once. 


The first law says precisely that there is a parameter called the internal 

energy and the change (A) in the internal energy of a system is equal to 

the heat, q f added to the system minus the work, w, that the system does 

on the environment. (The internal energy is usually written as U so as not 

to confuse it with the electrical potential.) In other words, the energy of a 

system is not determined directly, but rather by the difference between the 

heat and work that changes when the system changes. 


A u = q - w 


This is how thermodynamics is taught. To get to the next step you 

need to understand the idea of a state variable. A state variable is a vari¬ 

able where any change is path-independent. For example, the familiar 

temperature, T, and pressure, P, are state variables: It doesn't matter 

whether you change the temperature quickly or slowly because the 

effect on the system is controlled only by the difference between the 

temperature after the change minus the temperature before the change 

(AT). The usual analogy is the as-the-crow-flies geographical distance, 

say, between New York and San Francisco. This is a state variable: It 

doesn't matter whether you fly directly or go through Memphis and Salt 

Lake City (like the flights I wind up on)—all that matters is the final 

state you arrive in. 


The energy U is a state variable. Any process that you carry out will have 

a change in U that depends only on the initial and final states. However, q 

and w are NOT state variables (which is why they are written in lower case 

or with some other marking to distinguish them from state variables). How 

you design your machine will determine how much work you can get out 

of it and how much of the energy change will be wasted as heat. Looking 



138 



Nutrition in Crisis 



at the biological case: Two different metabolic changes, such as responses 

to different meals, might have the same total energy change, but the work 

(both physical and chemical) and heat you generate are likely to differ 

if the two meals have very different inefficiencies. There is no theoretical 

reason why they couldn’t have the same efficiency just by coincidence, but 

it is highly unlikely 


A preview of the second law is contained within the first law itself. The 

second law is what thermodynamics is really about—it is why thermo¬ 

dynamics took so long to evolve and why it is so hard to understand. The 

second law pretty much guarantees, contrary to popular misconception, 

that “a calorie is not a calorie.” 


Lets take a step back. There are four laws of thermodynamics, and the 

first law only operates in concert with the others. The zeroth law and the 

third law are pretty much theoretical, defining thermal equilibrium and the 

condition of absolute zero of temperature. It is the second law that embod¬ 

ies the special character of thermodynamics. Described by Ilya Prigogine, 

the Nobel prize-winning chemist and philosopher of thermodynamics, as 

the first revolutionary science, the second law explains how one diet can be 

more or less efficient that the other. The essential feature of the second law 

is the existence of a thing called entropy 



Entropy 


Entropy is a measure of how disorganized a system is—that is, what its 

possibilities are. As described below, it is a measure of information. A gas 

filling a box completely is said to have higher entropy than a gas that is 

confined to only one side of the box. Another description: You would not 

need a very good GPS to find out if the molecule is in New York City 

versus whether it is in Yankee Stadium. 


Entropy is traditionally defined with respect to a classic imaginary 

experiment. Although theoretical, it is clearly related to practical things. 

The experiment involves a creature referred to as the Maxwells demon, 

who is capable of doing things perfectly smoothly and slowly without 

exerting any energy or creating any heat from friction. The experimental 

apparatus is a box with a partition—a membrane that separates the box 

into two compartments, one that is filled with a gas and one that is empty. 



Beyond “A Calorie Is a Calorie” 



139 



Maxwells demon very carefully and slowly removes the membrane so 

that no work is done and no heat is generated. According to the first law 

of thermodynamics, which specifies the need to conserve energy and heat, 

nothing has really happened. In a real experiment, you could make an 

electronic device to open the partition so efficiently that it would hardly 

raise your electric bill—so effectively that energy is conserved and nothing 

should happen. Of course, you know that something will happen. The gas 

will fill up the whole box. Why? Because thats the way the world works 

according to the second law: Entropy will always increase and the gas will 

always spread as much as it can. 


The gas fills up the whole box because it is the most probable way for 

the molecules to be distributed. The second law is about probability. Before 

the nineteenth century, physics held a view of a universe that was mechani¬ 

cal, a universe standing alone in time and controllable. The second law 

suddenly brought out the irreversible and statistical nature of things. It is 

not that nobody knew that time passed before that point, but the idea of 

physical processes running down like a clock—the notion of irreversible 

processes—was revolutionary. The history of physics shows how hard it is 

for people, even very smart people, to understand and cope with the idea 

of probability. As it evolved, however, the second law became very practical 

and explained how energy can be used to do work and how chemical reac¬ 

tions occur. It is the key to understanding energy transformation in life. 



Entropy and Information 


Entropy is about information. It is frequently said that increased entropy 

means increased disorder. The more precise way to put it would be that 

increased entropy means a less demanding way of arranging a system, 

and in turn, higher probability. In poker, a straight flush has much lower 

entropy than two of a kind because there are fewer ways to get a straight 

flush. Here’s where it gets more complicated: Entropy—that is, informa¬ 

tion—can overpower energy. A receiver in American football can catch 

a pass even though he is double-teamed (his energy is compromised) 

because he has the information to know where the ball will be thrown. 

(The term entropy is also used in communications, where it indicates the 

extent to which a message has been corrupted during transmission.) 



140 



Nutrition in Crisis 



There are many ways to state the second law. One formal way says that 

it is impossible to carry out an operation where the sole effect is to transfer 

heat from a cold body to a warm body. This is obvious enough but it might 

not be easy to explain why. After all, the ocean is cold, but it has a huge 

amount of heat because it is so large. Why couldn’t a ship extract a little bit 

of that heat to run its engines? That’s the question Maxwell was really asking. 


The original creature proposed by Maxwell was more sophisticated 

than the demon described above. This original demon sat on a membrane 

separating two compartments. One compartment had a hot gas, and the 

other, a cold gas. The partition had a little door—a very well-machined, 

well-oiled door, whose openings and closings did not require any real 

amount of work and did not generate any heat. The demon would look 

into the cold gas and if he saw a fast-moving particle, he would open the 

door and let the particle into the hot compartment. He would, similarly, 

let slow-moving particles move from the hot gas to the cold gas. The net 

effect was to transfer heat from a cold body to a warm body, a clear no-no 

according to the second law. It was a thought experiment, but you really 

could make a super efficient door, so why couldn’t you make something 

at least close to this setup? The ocean is cold but it is so far from absolute 

zero and it is so big that there is so much heat, why couldn’t you make 

something like a Maxwell’s demon to get some of it to power your ocean 

liner? Was the second law not universal? 


This paradox completely stumped physicists of the nineteenth century. 

It’s because it’s about probability and randomness and they were not used 

to thinking like that. The answer to Maxwell’s paradox is that it is not really 

a paradox so much as another way of stating the second law. That’s how 

the world is. Information costs. You can’t make such a setup. If you could 

use some fancy electronic device to set up a sensor to distinguish between 

fast and slow molecules, it would still have to do work. The reason it gets 

confusing is that in the physical (real) world, we are only used to dealing 

with gross collections of things and averages. If we want to get down to 

single molecules, we have to run a machine to do it. The simpler way of 

saying it is that the reason that we can’t make anything perfectly efficient 

is because we can’t control individual molecules. 


The point of all this is to say that, if you’re going to carry out all the 

work, physical and chemical (that is, metabolic), using the energy from 




Beyond “A Calorie Is a Calorie” 



141 



food, then you can’t do it perfectly efficiently. Some part of the energy must 

be wasted. Of course, unlike an engineer, if you are trying to lose weight, 

the job of synthesizing and storing fat might be one that you want to run 

as inefficiently as possible. 



The Second Law and Metabolic Advantage 


There are many different ways of stating the second law, but one version 

emphasizes the fact that all real engines—in fact, all processes in the real 

world—are inherently inefficient—not just practically, not because you 

cant construct them so carefully that theres no friction, but theoretically, 

absolutely. There is no escape from inefficiency. The second law says that 

a perfect engine, living or otherwise, is not possible (unless you could get 

one to run at the mysterious temperature called absolute zero; the third 

law does give you that). 


Inefficiency depends on where you stand and what you are trying to 

accomplish. In human beings, keeping warm (that is, using food for heat) 

might not be considered inefficient but from the point of view of a machine 

that is trying to manufacture protein and other cell material, it is energy that 

is wasted. The heat generated in the processing of food is called either ther¬ 

mic effect of feeding (TEF) or diet-induced thermogenesis (DIT). (The old 

name was “specific dynamic action.”) These are a measure of energy wasted 

as heat. They are an expression of the inefficiency of the human machine. 

There are other ways to waste energy: for example, the so-called NEAT, 

nonexercise activity thermogenesis, which is the scientific name for fidget¬ 

ing. The measured TEF of different macronutrients is different: Protein is 

much greater than carbohydrate, which is greater than fat. In other words, 

metabolic advantage is a well-documented fact, and the extent to which 

small changes add up is only a question of how you do the experiment. If 

you have to make glucose through the process of gluconeogenesis, rather 

than getting it from the diet, you are going to waste energy. 



Beyond “Calories In = Calories Out” 


In chemical thermodynamics we want to know whether a chemical reaction 

can be made to produce energy, or whether we have to put in energy to make 



142 



Nutrition in Crisis 



the reaction go. As in the industrial revolution, we want to make an efficient 

chemical machine—we re thinking, of course, of a machine where you put in 

food and out comes enough energy to lift heavy weights, or more to the point, 

synthesize complicated chemicals like proteins. The key idea rests with iden¬ 

tifying the likelihood of a reaction going forward with the reactions ability to 

require or to give off energy. With the discussion of the second law behind 

us, we can guess that it is not about conservation. It is about dissipation of 

energy. The 4 kcal/g that we assign to the energy in carbohydrate is the energy 

exported from the reaction of oxidation to the environment. That’s what you 

do in chemical thermodynamics. Otherwise, all food would have zero calo¬ 

ries: The heat lost in oxidation is gained by the calorimeter. Calories in equals 

calories out, sure, but that literally leaves you with absolutely nothing. 


What comes next shows you how real thermodynamics works. It is not 

particularly mathematical. All you need is high school algebra. You can see 

the beauty of thermodynamics and how it can tell you, right off, that its 

not just calories in, calories out (commonly abbreviated CICO on social 

media). You may like it, but if you prefer to avoid the math, you can skip to 

the summary at the end. 


The second law says, in essence, that all (real) systems are inefficient. 

In practice, the law can be used to tell you whether a chemical reaction 

actually produces energy or, as we have said, whether you will have to put 

in energy to make it go. This is the key in chemistry (and living systems in 

general): Does the reaction proceed as written? 


When we write A B, we want to know whether the reaction will 

proceed from left to right spontaneously. “Spontaneously” means without 

the addition of energy. It does not mean fast, which is a separate question. 

The 4 kcal of energy that you measure in the calorimeter is both a measure 

of the tendency of the reaction to occur (oxidations generally go easily by 

themselves and produce energy) and also the maximum energy available to 

do work. They are very closely related, because although living systems do 

mechanical work, the main use of the energy is in chemical work: synthe¬ 

sizing metabolites and cell material. The second law leads to the definition 

of a number of different forms of the energy that are used under different 

conditions. The particular form of the energy that is used under conditions 

of constant temperature and pressure—the conditions in which biochem¬ 

ists typically work—is called the Gibbs free energy, which is almost always 



Beyond “A Calorie Is a Calorie” 



143 



abbreviated with the letter G, and whose change is written with the Greek 

delta, AG. The Gibbs free energy for a chemical is precisely defined as the 

maximum work you can get from running the reaction at constant tempera¬ 

ture and pressure, and it is identified with the tendency of the reaction to go 

in the forward direction. The 4 kcal produced by the oxidation of glucose 

tells you that is the most you could get out of it in terms of work or driving 

other chemical reactions. In practice, some energy might be wasted as heat 

or other unproductive processes. 


To summarize what we have just discussed: Chemical thermodynamics 

emphasizes the reaction of the system, not the whole universe. We want 

to know about the energy exchange when we burn food. The complete 

oxidation of glucose in the calorimeter produces 4 kcal. It is not about 

conservation. It is about dissipation of energy 


The word thermodynamics is thrown around a lot in nutrition, mostly by 

people who have no idea what it s about. Again, you don t need thermody¬ 

namics to do nutrition, but if youre going to bring thermodynamics into 

the picture, you have to do it right. So, in case you want to see what people 

really do in chemical thermodynamics, I will present a good example. 


Let s begin with energy change, meaning Gibbs free energy Here we have 

a grand rule: If the free energy change is negative (AG < 0) for the reaction, 

the reaction is downhill, will go by itself, and will give off energy (which you 

might be able to capture by coupling it to another chemical reaction or to 

some mechanical, electrical, or heat machine).The Gibbs free energy has two 

components, the heat of reaction, called enthalpy (A#) and the entropy (AS). 


Here's a simple example of what you might do in real thermodynamics, 

which will also illustrate why the calorie equivalence idea in nutrition is 

not right. Suppose that you wanted to know about the formation of carbon 

monoxide (CO)—if carbon is oxidized to CO, how much energy is given 

off in the process. Generally thought of in the context of a poison, CO has 

other uses—small amounts are actually produced in the human body. So, 

we want to know: Is the oxidation of carbon to CO uphill or downhill and 

by how much? To keep it simple, well take the enthalpy (heat of reaction, 

A H). We can do the experiment so that the entropy is not an important 

player (low temperature). The heat of reaction is easily measured. 


In the case of oxidizing carbon, then, if heat is given off (AH = (-)), 

the reaction will be spontaneous and go by itself. The problem with trying 



144 



Nutrition in Crisis 



to figure out how much energy you can get by burning carbon to carbon 

monoxide is that you cant really measure it. If you try to carry out the 

reaction, you always get some CO 2 . So, what can you do? Here’s how to 

deal with it: We can’t measure the heat of reaction for oxidation of carbon 

to CO directly (again, because it is always complicated by some CO 2 being 

formed), but we can measure the enthalpy of burning of carbon to CO 2 (a 

minus sign means heat is given off): 


C + O 2 CO 2 AH = -94 kcal 


We also know the energy of burning CO to CO 2 : 


CO + V 2 0 2 -> C0 2 AH = -68 kcal 


Another great simplifying feature of thermodynamics is that if we know 

the energy for a chemical reaction (or any process), the energy for going 

the other way is the same numerically, only with the opposite sign, so: 


C0 2 -> CO + % 0 2 AH = +68 kcal 


The energy functions, G and H, are state functions. Remember from 

earlier in this chapter that means they are path-independent, or independent 

of the conditions under which a particular reaction is carried. State functions 

can be added just as in simple algebra. The energies add up (figure 9.3). Here 

you see the beauty of thermodynamics. The attraction to those of us who like 

it is that you can manipulate the results with elementary algebra. The great 

simplicity in this kind of calculation reflects its highly predictive power. 


What did we do? We had two different paths from carbon to carbon 

monoxide: one (two-step) path that we could calculate, and one that we are 

trying to find out. We knew they must be equal. The principle that allows 

you to add up heats of reaction is called Hess’s Law. 



Hess’s Law Shows That a Calorie Is Not a Calorie 


The idea that “a calorie is a calorie,” means that the energy yield for 

metabolism will be path-independent—that is, the same for all diets and 



Beyond “A Calorie Is a Calorie 



145 





C + 0 2 



AH = ? 



> CO 




c + o 2 -► co + y 2 o 2 



AH = -110.5 kj 





Protein 




■> Carbohydrate 




Figure 9.3. Hess’s Law. A, Calculating heat of reaction for formation of CO. Path of the 

arrows (measured] must equal the direct conversion to CO, so we just add them up. 

B, According to Hess’s Law (adding up energies), the energy for path 1, AG, should be 

equal to the energy for path 3 followed by path 2, A G^ - AG 2 . Using calorimeter values 

and the principle that a calorie is a calorie leads to a contradiction. Note: Energies in 

figure in kj = 4.28 kcal. 



proportional to the calorimeter values. To show that this is not true in 

general we use the simple additive property of state variables as in the 

carbon monoxide problem above. We will look at oxidation of protein by 









146 



Nutrition in Crisis 



two pathways, either directly or indirectly by first converting the protein to 

glucose through gluconeogenesis (GNG) and then oxidizing the glucose 

to CO 2 . The laws of thermodynamics say that these should be the same. 


The calorimeter values say that energy yield for carbohydrate and for 

protein are equivalent: AG (oxidation) = -4 kcal/mol (figure 9.2). The 

(-) sign, again, means energy is given off and the process is spontaneous, 

Here’s the plan. We make multiple paths for oxidizing protein: path 1 

(direct), and paths 2 and 3 (indirect): 


Path 1: Protein + 02 “^ CO 2 + H 2 O AGi = 4 kcal/g 


Path 2: Carbohydrate + O 2 ^ CO 2 + H 2 O AG 2 = 4 kcal/g 


Path 3 : Protein—GNC -> Carbohydrate + O 2 ^ CO 2 + H 2 0 AG 3 + AG 2 = ? 


In path 1, we burn protein directly to CO 2 . Because free energy is a 

state variable, the free energy AGi must be equal to the sum of AG 2 , the 

energy for path 2, plus AG 3 , the energy for path 3. This means that AG 3 

for path 3 must be about zero. However, path 3 includes the process of 

gluconeogenesis. Chemistry students work very hard in order to learn that 

gluconeogenesis is an expensive, endergonic (energy requiring) process: It 

costs energy (about 6 ATP) to turn a mole of protein into a mole of carbo¬ 

hydrate. It is the failure to take this into account—the assumption that 

only the calories measured in a calorimeter are important—that leads to a 

contradiction. Once you take into account the real metabolic processes, it 

becomes clear that “a calorie is a calorie” is not a valid principle. The exam¬ 

ple bears on the real behavior of living systems, and is likely a contributor 

to the benefits of dietary carbohydrate restriction. More generally, if one 

diet is reported to be more effective for weight loss than another, there is 

no reason not to take it seriously. Like any scientific result, it should be 

evaluated carefully, but if it is your body mass that you are concerned about, 

the stakes are high. 


The Nonequilibrium Picture 


There is one more level of sophistication to address. To some extent, it is 

not really about thermodynamics at all, or at least not equilibrium ther¬ 

modynamics (the energy required to go between states in equilibrium). 



Beyond “A Calorie Is a Calorie” 



147 




Breakfast Lunch Dinner 


Meal 


Figure 9.4. Hypothetical model for the effect of insulin on efficiency of storage. The 

lower line, indicating response under conditions of weight maintenance, fluctuates 

but averages to no change. The upper line shows the effect of added insulin on 

hormone-sensitive lipase activity. The important point is that the energy differences 

are very small compared to the equilibrium value, which would be very far below the 

figure. The system is not controlled by energy but by rates. 



Equilibrium thermodynamics is what is usually studied, and we are taught 

that rates of reaction are considered separately from energy Equilibrium 

thermodynamics tell, for example, that amino acids are more stable than 

proteins. The rate of breakdown is very slow. If you could keep the bacteria 

off your steak it would last for months, or even years, but at the end of 

time it would be all amino acids, (or even simpler things). In biochem¬ 

istry, however, rates become important because living systems are not at 

equilibrium until they die. Things are moving forward. All the reactions in 

biology are run by catalysts—that is, enzymes that control the speed of a 

reaction, rather than the energetics. A better way to put it might be to say 

that the key players in all this are hormones and hormones generally affect 

enzymes, which in turn affect rates, not energy. 


Living systems are not at equilibrium. Living systems, in fact, main¬ 

tain themselves very far from equilibrium. They are characterized by an 

in-and-out flux of material and energy. In a dietary intervention, material 

fluctuates around a level far from equilibrium. In other words, changes 








148 



Nutrition in Crisis 



with time become important and changes might be controlled by the pres¬ 

ence of catalysts—enzymes or other factors that affect the rate of reaction. 


Figure 9.4 shows the theoretical fluctuations of fat within a fat cell. 

The key idea is that the reactions, breakdown, and resynthesis of the fat 

molecule are very far from equilibrium (at equilibrium, you would have 

very little fat, mostly fatty acid, and free glycerol). Looking at fat gain and 

loss (figure 9.4), adipocytes cycle between states of greater or lesser net 

breakdown of fat (lipolysis and reformation) depending on the hormonal 

state, which in turn is dependent on the macronutrient composition of 

the diet. A hypothetical scheme for changes in adipocyte TAG and a 

proposal for how TAG gain or loss could be different for isocaloric diets 

with different levels of insulin is shown in the figure. The basic idea is that 

fat fluctuates, but if you slowly store enough so that the fat molecules you 

form don't have a chance to break down before you consume another meal, 

fat will accumulate regardless of caloric input. 


Under normal control conditions of weight maintenance, the break¬ 

down and utilization of TAG follows a pattern of lipolysis (fasting), and 

intake plus resynthesis (meals). To make it simple, let’s assume a sudden, 

instantaneous spike in food at meals. Then the curves represent the net 

flow of material within the adipocyte. This averages out to a stable weight 

maintenance (lower dotted line in figure 9.4). If we keep each meal at 

constant calories but increase the percentage of carbohydrate or otherwise 

generate a higher insulin level, the hormone-sensitive lipases, the enzymes 

that catalyze the breakdown of fat, will be inhibited (the solid line in figure 

9.4). Resynthesis of TAG is thus less affected by the elevated insulin and 

might actually slow down. The net effects are changes in the direction 

of accumulation of TAG. It is theoretical, but the model shows you how 

kinetics (how fast things happen) might be more important than thermo¬ 

dynamics (how stable they are at the end of the reaction). 



PART 3 


The Low- 


Carbohydrate Diet 

for Disease 




-CHAPTER 10- 



Diabetes 



At the end of our clinic day, we go home thinking, “The 

clinical improvements are so large and obvious, why don't 

other doctors understand?” Carbohydrate-restriction is easily 

grasped by patients: because carbohydrates in the diet raise 

the blood glucose, and as diabetes is defined by high blood 

glucose, it makes sense to lower the carbohydrate in the diet . 

By reducing the carbohydrate in the diet, we have been able to 

taper patients off as much as 150 units of insulin per day in 

8 days, with marked improvement in glycemic control—even 

normalization of glycemicparameters . 


—Eric Westman 1 



T he scene is lunch in the cafeteria at SUNY Downstate Medical 

Center. I am going on about how strange it is that carbohydrate 

is recommended for people with diabetes, a disease whose most 

salient symptom is high blood glucose. Overhearing my story, several 

clinicians at the table ask incredulously, almost in unison, “Who gives 

carbohydrates to diabetics?” I say, “The American Diabetes Association 

(ADA). They recommend 55-60 percent carbs (or whatever their values 

were at the time).” Their response? I believe the cliche is “deafening 

silence.” Its true. The ADA recommends high-carbohydrate diets to be 

compensated for with medication. Sugar is okay as long as you “cover with 

insulin.”That was really their recommendation, at least in 2010: “Sucrose- 

containing foods can be substituted for other carbohydrates in the meal 

plan or, if added to the meal plan, covered with insulin or other glucose 

lowering medications.”In 2013 they finally, and quite quietly, dropped this 

bizarre and ultimately harmful recommendation, but it remains a testa¬ 

ment to their willingness to recommend a dietary treatment that will make 



152 



Nutrition in Crisis 



things worse so that more drugs can be taken, or more accurately, because 

more drugs can be taken. 


The folks at the table were not endocrinologists and they were of my 

generation. They grew up knowing that you don’t give carbohydrates to 

people with diabetes. Simple. After all, it will increase their blood sugar, 

which is precisely what you are trying to prevent in those who have the 

disease. Because it was not their medical specialization, the clinicians were 

unaware that recommendations had evolved at official agencies, or even 

that it is now politically incorrect to refer to patients as “diabetics” (unless 

you yourself are a person with diabetes). They believed in the old common- 

sense idea that because diabetes was a disease of carbohydrate intolerance, 

cutting out carbohydrate should be the first line of attack—and they prob¬ 

ably knew that it worked. Why wouldn’t it work? 



Diabetes: Type 1 and Type 2 


Diabetes is a disease—several diseases, really—of carbohydrate intolerance. 

In type 1 the intolerance is due to the inability of the pancreas to produce 

insulin in response to carbohydrate, and in type 2 it is due to poor cellular 

response to the insulin that is produced, accompanied by deterioration of 

the insulin-producing cells of the pancreas. The most salient symptom of 

diabetes (and a major contributor to the pathology) is high blood sugar, 

which, not surprisingly, is most effectively treated by reducing dietary 

carbohydrates. The common clinical measurements are: (1) fasting blood 

glucose (sometimes written FBG), usually given in units of milligrams per 

deciliter (mg/dL) and normal considered to be around 100 mg/dL; (2) an 

oral glucose tolerance test (sometimes abbreviated OGTT), the response 

in the blood to a dose of glucose; and (3) the percent of hemoglobin Ale 

(HbAlc), a modified form of hemoglobin (that has reacted with blood 

glucose) that is a measure of the cumulative effect of the high blood sugar. 

Normal levels for the latter are about 5 percent, but people with diabetes 

can have values of 20 percent. In type 1, patients are required to inject insu¬ 

lin, whereas people with type 2 are advised to improve their lifestyle “with 

diet and exercise.” If those with type 2 cannot lower their blood sugar with 

diet—the diet that their doctor recommends is unlikely to adequately do 

so—they will also require insulin or other drugs for reduction of glucose, 



Diabetes 



153 



of which there are many. The insulin level of 150 units per day cited in the 

introductory quotation to this chapter is a very high dose. Coming off such 

a high dose is a major accomplishment, suggesting that Eric Westmans 

diet might be better than the standard, and of course, reduction in medica¬ 

tion is considered improvement in all the diseases that I know of. 



Why Diabetes? 


Diabetes stands at the forefront of the nutrition problem because it’s 

so clearly linked to control of metabolism and the glucose-insulin axis. 

Correspondingly, it shows exactly what carbohydrate restriction can do 

for you. Diabetes is, in a real sense, the extreme version of the nutritional 

problem in obesity and possibly heart disease. Although Atkins or other 

low-carbohydrate diets are used as a therapy for overweight or obesity, they 

are not just about weight loss. In fact, they are not even primarily about 

weight loss, despite the fact that they are more frequently used by people 

who are overweight than by those with diabetes. Many diets work for weight 

loss, and calorie restriction may or may not be part of the reason that a 

low-carbohydrate diet makes you lose weight, but to bring diabetes under 

control, we know that you have to reduce carbohydrate regardless of whether 

or not calories go down, and regardless of whether or not you lose weight. 


The intuitive idea that people with diabetes should not consume much 

sugar or starch is a good principle. Nothing in the science contradicts this. 

However effective a diet is for treatment, nobody really knows what is 

required for prevention because it is often a kind of hidden disease. Early 

symptoms might be low-level, such as simple irritability and fatigue, and 

type 2, in particular, can have a very slow onset. As a treatment, however, 

a low-carbohydrate diet is better than drugs for most people. This is 

knowledge we cannot afford to ignore, considering how terrible a disease 

diabetes is: It is the major cause of acquired blindness and the major cause 

of amputations after accidents. 



Where Does It Come From? 


In chapter 5,1 described Claude Bernard’s revolutionary discovery of the 

liver’s control of blood glucose and its regulation of glycogen turnover and 



154 



Nutrition in Crisis 



gluconeogenesis (GNG). Bernard voiced astonishment at finding sugar 

in a dog that hadn’t consumed any sugar. Historians suspect, however, 

that writing about it later he might have exaggerated how much the 

original observations really took him by surprise. He wouldn’t be the first 

and undoubtedly will not be the last scientist to revise the history of his 

discoveries for dramatic effect. It makes for a great story to have the answer 

fall from heaven, especially if you can describe the intervention of your 

prepared mind, as Pasteur put it. 


It is likely that Claude Bernard had suspected for a long time that 

animals could make their own sugar. He guessed that it could be made 

from fat—which was not true—or from protein—which was true. One clue 

that would have led him in this direction was his observation that people 

with diabetes had more glucose in their blood than could be accounted for 

by dietary intake alone. He must have suspected that those people with 

diabetes were making their own sugar. In fact, gluconeogenesis goes on all 

the time in the body, whether or not you have diabetes. GNG is not, as the 

textbooks sometimes imply, a last-ditch resource during starvation, after 

glycogen is depleted. 


GNG goes on all the time. As described in chapter 5, when you wake 

up in the morning, half of your blood glucose might come from GNG. 

The difference for people with diabetes is that they have lost the ability 

to turn it off at the right time. This is due to the reduction or absence of 

insulin, as in type 1, or the poor response to the insulin that occurs in 

type 2. In healthy individuals, the presence of glucose coming in from 

the diet causes insulin secretion, which then inhibits gluconeogenesis and 

glycogen breakdown. Many people think that the high blood sugar seen 

in diabetes is due to a failure in clearance because the cells cannot take 

up the glucose in the blood for fuel. Even the textbooks make this claim. 

While the number of GLUT4 receptors in people with diabetes does 

not increase in response to dietary glucose as much as it does in healthy 

people, it seems that this is not the major cause of hyperglycemia—people 

with diabetes still have enough of these glucose-transporting receptors 

under most conditions. 


The major problem, in fact, appears to be the persistence of glucose 

production from the liver. Under normal conditions, release of glucose 

from liver glycogen and gluconeogenesis are both regulated by insulin and 



Diabetes 



155 



glucagon. As glucose goes up, insulin goes up and glucagon goes down. 

In response, the liver stops producing additional glucose. A key feature of 

diabetes is the loss of this stimulus-response control over glucose produc¬ 

tion. With that in mind, it does not make sense to recommend dietary 

glucose to people with diabetes, as it will simply sit on top of the unregu¬ 

lated level of built-in production. 



Can You Treat It? 


Dietary carbohydrate restriction has been a therapy for diabetes since 

before the discovery of insulin. 2 It is not a new idea that blood glucose 

control has positive effects on all of the downstream effects of the 

disease, including lipid markers for, and incidence of, cardiovascular 

disease. For many diabetes sufferers, dietary carbohydrate restriction is 

all that is needed to improve or eliminate symptoms. Normal glucose 

control, stable weight, and normal lipids can be attained, and the 

benefits persist as long as carbohydrate intake is low. It then becomes a 

semantic question whether someone can be called “cured” if they have 

to stay on a prescribed diet indefinitely. The real question is whether a 

low-carbohydrate diet, or the high-carbohydrate diet recommended by 

health agencies, the one that was associated with onset of the diabetes, 

is the more extreme. 


A key issue is the established association between body weight and type 

2 diabetes, and whether the association is causal. Studies by researchers at 

the University of Minnesota provide strong evidence against the idea that 

there is a causal link between the two. Nuttall and Gannon have produced 

a series of well-designed, well-controlled experiments demonstrating the 

value of carbohydrate restriction in treating diabetes under conditions 

where no weight is lost. They found that carbohydrate restriction has a 

consistent and dramatic ability to reduce high blood glucose, even when 

there is no change in body mass. It is not easy to lose weight, so the abil¬ 

ity to treat diabetes without concern for weight loss provides an obvious 

advantage. From a theoretical standpoint, Nuttall and Gannon’s studies 

support the idea that both obesity and diabetes are reflections of a central 

cause—disruptions in the glucose-insulin axis, most likely. In other words, 

obesity and diabetes might stand in a parallel, rather than serial, relation to 



156 



Nutrition in Crisis 



each other. The most obvious evidence to support this idea is the fact that 

there are people with diabetes, both types 1 and 2, who are not fat, and of 

course, most fat people do not have diabetes. 


The obstinate refusal of endocrinologists and diabetes educators to face 

these ideas, and hardest to understand, their reluctance to face Nuttall 

and Gannons experimental results, remains a major obstacle in dealing 

with the current epidemic. Almost every statement, whether from health 

agencies or individual experts, continues to emphasize weight loss as the 

prime goal of diabetes treatment, and endocrinologists continue to make 

this their recommendation. The problem is that the scientific evidence on 

the subject is just not on their radar. Why not? Endocrinologists already 

have to retain so much medical knowledge that its not entirely surprising 

that they dont know much about nutrition. There is no excuse, however, 

for the endocrinologists who act as if they know nutrition but refuse to 

consider low-carbohydrate diets. I doubt that they love their patients any 

less, but perhaps they love hating Dr. Atkins more. 


Low-GI versus 

Real Low-Carbohydrate 


If low-GI isgood y how about no-GI? 


—Eric Westman 


In 2008, David Jenkins compared a diet high in cereal with a low- 

glycemic index diet. 3 As I explained in chapter 2, the glycemic index 

is a measure of the actual effect of dietary glucose on blood glucose. 

Pioneered by Jenkins and coworkers, a low-GI diet is based on the same 

rationale as a low-carbohydrate diet: that glycemic and insulin fluctua¬ 

tions pose a metabolic risk. GI emphasizes “the type of carbohydrate,” 

thus offering a politically correct form of low-carbohydrate diet. As 

stated in the 2008 study: 


We selected a high cereal fiber diet treatment for its suggested health 

benefits for the comparison so that the potential value of carbohy¬ 

drate foods could be emphasized equally for both high cereal fiber 

and low—glycemic index interventions. (Emphasis added) 



Diabetes 



157 



After the completion of the twenty-four-week studyjenkins concluded: 

“In patients with type 2 diabetes, 6-month treatment with a low-glycemic 

index diet resulted in moderately lower HbAlc levels compared with a 

high-cereal fiber diet.” Coincidentally, on almost the same day that David 

Jenkins’s study came out, Eric Westman’s group published a study that 

compared a low-GI diet with a true low-carbohydrate diet. 4 The stud¬ 

ies were comparable in duration and number of subjects, and a direct 

comparison (figure 10.1) shows that the true low-carbohydrate has much 

greater benefits—hence the quotation from Dr. Westman at the head of 

this section. 


Figure 10.1 by itself constitutes the best evidence for a low-carbohydrate 

diet as the “default diet,” the one to try first, for diabetes and metabolic 



Weight 


(kg) 



HbAlc 

[% x 10] 



Glucose 


[mg/dL] 



Total-C 


[mg/dL] 



LDL 


[mg/dL] 



HDL 


[mg/dL] 




TG 


[mg/dL] 





-30 



-40 



-50 



-60 



Jenkins High-Cereal Fiber Diet 

Jenkins Low-Glycemic Index Diet 


| Westman Low-Glycemic Index Diet 


| Westman Low-Carbohydrate, 

Ketogenic Diet 



-70 



Figure 10.1. Effect of high-cereal fiber or low-glycemic index diets on body weight and 

hHbAlc. Data from D. J. Jenkins et al., "Effect of a Low-Glycemic Index or a High-Cereal Fiber 

Diet on Type 2 Diabetes: A Randomized Trial "Journo! of the American Medical Association 

300, no. 23 [2008]: 2742-2753; E. C. Westman et al., “The Effect of a Low-Carbohydrate, 

Ketogenic Diet Versus a Low-Glycemic Index Diet on Glycemic Control in Type 2 Diabetes 

Mellitus" Nutrition and Metabolism 5, no. 36 (2008). 




158 



Nutrition in Crisis 



syndrome. There are hundreds of studies about all aspects of diabetes but 

none contradict the benefits of carbohydrate reduction shown in figure 

10.1. It is worth noting, however, that one additional benefit wasn’t 

addressed by Westman’s study: Low-carbohydrate diet reliably reduces 

the dependence on drugs. In William Yancy’s classic study 5 of twenty-one 

patients on low-carbohydrate ketogenic diets, all but four reduced their 

level of medication and seven patients discontinued medication altogether. 

Reducing medication, in and of itself, is a huge advance. Is that it? That’s 

it. The hundreds of papers published provide a variety of details about this 

very complicated disease and its response to the numerous drugs that are 

used as therapy, but the basic principles described here have never been 

refuted in any fundamental way. 


Diabetes is a central battleground in the new low-carbohydrate revolu¬ 

tion and it is likely to provide a major victory. Scientifically, the burden of 

proof is on those who continue to claim that it is a good idea for people with 

diabetes to consume any significant amount of carbohydrate. Impressive 

as the comparison shown in figure 10.1 is, it has made little impact. 

Establishment medicine has arbitrarily decided that long-term randomized 

controlled trials (RCTs) are a kind of “gold standard.” Whatever their value 

for particular types of experiments, this principle is used to ignore any other 

type of study, especially if it challenges the party line. RCT studies can be 

useful, but the people who insist on using them for diabetes sit on panels 

that would never fund an RCT study if it included low-carbohydrate diets. 

Moreover, an RCT is not the best thing for everything. It is one kind of 

experiment, and all possibilities are not up for grabs in science. Since diabetes 

is primarily about carbohydrate, there is no better treatment than reducing 

carbohydrate. There is nothing in the outcome of low-carbohydrate trials of 

whatever length to suggest harm from such a therapeutic regimen—only 

benefit. Absolute dependence on arbitrary rules and “gold standards” is 

what continues to cause the most harm. 



What About Cardiovascular Disease? 


The “concerns” about low-carbohydrate diets still revolve around the 

imagined risk of cardiovascular disease from fat in the diet despite the 



Diabetes 



159 



continued failure to show any such risk. Carbohydrate restriction actually 

improves the usual markers of CVD—notably HDL (“good cholesterol”) 

and triglycerides. Although there haven't yet been long-term trials to show 

that carbohydrate reduction prevents CVD, there are plenty of long-term 

trials on the effect of reducing fat, and they all fail to show any clinically 

significant effect. 


There is a strong association between diabetes and the incidence of 

CVD, but the largely discredited, or at least greatly exaggerated, diet-heart 

hypothesis—and its proscriptions against dietary fat—has caused us to 

ignore the carbohydrate elephant in the room. It turns out, however, that 

the best predictor of microvascular complications (blindness, amputa¬ 

tions) and, to a lesser extent, macrovascular complications (heart attack, 

stroke) in patients with type 2 diabetes is hemoglobin Ale, which is under 

the control of chronic dietary carbohydrate consumption. Data from 

the United Kingdom Prevention of Diabetes Study (UKPDS) 6 provide 

support for this idea. In some sense, increased risk of CVD for people 

with diabetes is due simply to the diabetes itself. More precisely, the same 

conditions that gave rise to the diabetic state—disruptions in the glucose- 

insulin axis—increase the risk of vascular disease. 



Summary 


Diabetes is a disease of carbohydrate intolerance. Type 1 is character¬ 

ized by an inability to produce insulin in response to carbohydrate, and 

in type 2, there is peripheral insulin resistance along with deterioration 

of the beta cells of the pancreas. The most salient symptom of diabetes 

(and a major contributor to the pathology) is high blood sugar, which, 

not surprisingly, is most effectively treated by reducing dietary carbo¬ 

hydrates. The clinical measurements are: (1) fasting blood glucose; (2) 

an oral glucose tolerance test, the response in the blood to a dose of 

glucose; and (3) the percent of modified hemoglobin, hemoglobin Ale 

(HbAlc). It is not hard to guess the best treatment for a disease char¬ 

acterized by poor response to ingested carbohydrates, but you do need 

to know if the theory will hold up in practice. A personal story might 

bring this into perspective. 



160 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Wendy's Story 


The Uses of Metabolic Adversity 


By Wendy Pogozelski 


Wendy Pogozelski is professor and chairman of biochem¬ 

istry at SUNY Genesee in upstate New York. One of 

the people who has used low-carbohydrate diets to teach 

metabolism , she developed type i diabetes as an adult . In 

2012 , ASBMB Today, the house organ ofthe American 

Society of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology* started a 

series about challenges to biochemists, Wendys story was 

their first publication in this series: 


Blurred vision was the first sign that something was wrong. The 

front row of the freshman chemistry class I was teaching looked 

strangely fuzzy. Then, over the next few days, I was gripped by 

an unquenchable thirst and was constandy fatigued. Seemingly 

overnight I lost eight pounds. I recognized the symptoms of 

diabetes, but I was young(ish), slim(ish) and an avid kickboxer. 

Mine was not the typical diabetic profile. 


Despite my suspicion that I was experiencing raging hyper¬ 

glycemia, the diagnosis—“You have diabetes 7 ’—was devastating. 

It marked the beginning of a lifestyle that is an enormous 

challenge. However, the journey has led me to an increased 

understanding of biochemistry, has enhanced my teaching 

and ultimately has cast me in a new role of helping others. It 

turned out that I had developed latent autoimmune diabetes in 

adults, or LADA, a subset of type 1 diabetes. LADA is due to 

an autoimmune reaction to pancreatic glutamate decarboxylase, 

or GAD65. While LADA has a slower onset than classic type 

1, formerly known as juvenile diabetes, the two diseases follow 

a similar course and require injections of insulin. 



Diabetes 



161 



Fortunately, I felt equipped to manage my condition. I teach 

metabolism to undergraduates using an approach that empha¬ 

sizes insulin-dependent pathways as a unifying theme and one 

that offers an everyday context. I knew that carbohydrates, 

whether whole grain or highly processed, could raise my blood 

glucose to dangerous levels, so my response to the diagnosis was 

to reduce greatly carbohydrates in my diet. In addition, I was 

careful to monitor my blood sugar levels and insulin doses. Hie 

result was that my hemoglobin Ale (glycosylated hemoglobin, 

a measure of blood sugar control) was 5.4 percent, within the 

normal 4 percent to 5 percent range. My doctor said that I was 

his “best patient ever 7 * and that I was achieving the blood sugars 

of a nondiabetic person. 


Despite satisfaction with my glycemic control, my physician 

wanted me to see a dietitian. To my surprise, the dietitian was 

appalled by my diet. She said, “You have to eat a minimum of 

130 grams of carbohydrates a day/T protested, but she recruited 

the rest of the medical team to endorse her position: “We all say 

you have to eat more earbs.The American Diabetes Association 

gives us these guidelines/ 5 One member of the team said, “I 

want you to eat chocolate. I want you to enjoy life.” 


As someone raised to be cooperative, and because I found it 

easy to embrace medical advice to eat chocolate, I agreed to eat 

more carbohydrates. The result was that my HbAlc rose above 

7 percent. My blood sugar levels were frequently in the 200 to 

300 mg/dL range (far above the normal level of about 85 mg/ 

dL), even when I supplemented with extra insulin. My former 

dose of seven units of insulin per day increased to 30 units per 

day. The loss of control was immensely frustrating. My physi¬ 

cian attributed my initial success to what is called the diabetes 

honeymoon. Often, when someone first begins taking insulin, 

there is a short-lived period during which beta cells seem to 

recover a bit and secrete insulin. Regardless, it was clear that the 




162 



Nutrition in Crisis 



dietitians approach was not yielding the success I desired. I felt 

confused and uncertain as to what to do. 


I decided to investigate for myself what my best diet should 

be. I studied the literature, I sought out researchers and physi¬ 

cians, and I attended countless metabolism-related talks. In 

addition, I connected with hundreds of people with diabetes. 

The most important contribution to my achieving clarity, 

however, was evaluating literature based on a molecular under¬ 

standing of how metabolism works. 


In my quest for answers, I found to my surprise that many 

dietitians and physicians were unable to explain the basis for the 

dietary recommendations they endorsed. Some did, however, 

express a desire for a better understanding or review of what 

they'd once learned. And in the general public, I encountered 

scores of diabetics and nondiabetics who also wanted tools to 

make sense of conflicting nutritional information. 


I began to use what I had learned not only to expand and 

improve my teaching and research but also to step into the role 

of a nutrition explainer. First I was determined to see that none 

of my students would lack understanding of processes such as 

gluconeogenesis and the many pathways affected by insulin. I 

created new lecture topics and problem sets based on diabetes 

and nutrition applications. My students responded positively 

and appreciatively. There was a palpable increase in attention 

in class. 


Students came to my office to chat about things that they had 

read. My class evaluations praised the use of nutritional context 

and often said, a This material could have been rather dull withi 

out 'all these great applications.” I even heard (frequently) “I 

love metabolism!” 


Beyond my student population, I engaged a world of 

bloggers, physicians and other people with diabetes, many of 

whom were eager to understand more deeply how things work 



Diabetes 



163 



metabolically. I now find myself being interviewed, quoted 

in papers, and invited to speak to groups of people, includ¬ 

ing physicians, who want to deepen their understanding of 

metabolic pathways. I am asked to share my nutrition-based 

teaching applications with other professors and with textbook 

publishers. In these efforts, I try to avoid dispensing nutritional 

advice; instead, I attempt to show how nutrient composition 

affects metabolic pathways so that my audience feels better able 

to evaluate nutritional recommendations. 


Five years later, diabetes is still an immense mental and 

physical challenge, but I am grateful for the insight and tools 

that my education and training have provided me. Most 

importantly, if I am able to further the use of molecular 

science to help others find optimal dietary strategies, and if I 

can help the next generation, then my adversity will have had 

a positive outcome. 


Epilogue 


The story in ASBMB Today was written for a series on how 

scientists overcome hard times rather than as a treatise on how 

to manage diabetes. However, the essay was read by many folks 

who were interested from a standpoint of their own health 

concerns. I received many requests for an update, as people 

wondered if, after my foray into inclusion of carbohydrates, I 

returned to my low-carbohydrate style of eating. The answer is 

a resounding “yes.” (With one small caveat, as you 11 see below.) 


As I studied the scientific literature, I became more and more 

convinced that my absolute primary concern should be keeping 

my blood sugar levels as close to normal as possible. I saw that 

I achieve the flattest blood sugars when I keep my carbohydrate 

low and my insulin low. Dr. Richard Bernstein, a type 1 diabetic 

and engineer-turned-physician who pioneered the use of 

glucose meters, calls this principle “the law of small numbers.” 




164 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Carbohydrate restriction results in minimization of errors. 

Hyperglycemia and hypoglycemia come from mis-estimation 

of carbohydrate amount, rate of carbohydrate absorption and 

insulin absorption and activity. These factors are nearly impos¬ 

sible to predict accurately. Many people who use large amounts 

of insulin to cover large amounts of carbohydrate in their diet 

frequently find themselves in dangerous situations (passing out, 

etc.) when the peak activity of the insulin occurs earlier than 

the peak absorption of the dietary carbohydrate. I have never 

passed out and my health care team has been astounded by my 

lack of hypoglycemic episodes. Hyperglycemia also needs to be 

avoided though. It is these high blood sugars that correlate with 

the long-term deleterious effects of diabetes. Observing the 

diabetic amputees at the endocrinologists office and watching 

people painfully shuffle into the dialysis center has been strong 

motivation to avoid these high blood sugars. 


What is the evidence for the effectiveness of a low-carb 

dietary approach with me? I was used as a test subject for 

“sensor"’ technology. This device consists of a needle, sensor 

and a transmitter worn on the body and it communicates with 

an insulin pump. When my results were printed out after the 

experiment, my health care team was astonished at how level 

my glucose readings were. I was the last appointment of the day 

and all the workers gathered around to admire the printout of 

the “beautiful” blood sugars and ask me how in the world I did 

that. (This was at the office where the team had insisted that I 

needed to eat carbohydrate. Yes, it was very satisfying.) 


It is worth noting too how well having level blood sugar 

makes me feel. When my blood sugar goes over 200 mg/dL, 

I start to feel melodramatic and have an elevated emotional 

response to even minor difficulties. Low blood sugar induces 

glucagon and epinephrine hormone release, which result in 

sweating and a feeling of panic as well as low energy Both 



Diabetes 



165 



high and low blood sugar make my brain sluggish. Also, as a 

headache-prone person for much of my life, I found that my 

formerly frequent headaches nearly disappeared when I began 

to practice carbohydrate restriction. 


When I keep my meal at less than 10 g of carbohydrate, 

my blood sugars rise by no more than 40 mg/dL; sometimes 

they rise as little as 10 mg/dL, but regardless, they return to 

normal within two hours—like a nondiabetic, I find that too 

much protein will elevate my blood sugar so I keep that amount 

moderate but I don’t consciously restrict it, or my fat. 


Despite my knowledge that carbohydrate restriction is best 

for me, I sometimes have deviated from this plan of attack. 

Particularly, since I became a mom, there has been a lot more 

carbohydrate in the kitchen tempting me. Couple that with 

the chronic exhaustion of motherhood and you’ve got a situa¬ 

tion that makes reaching for carbohydrate much more likely. I 

rediscovered that M&Ms- (used effectively for potty-training a 

toddler) are delicious, and my plans to eat no more than three 

have consistently been shown to be no match for whatever else 

is at work. (My theory is that there is an evil force activated 

when one eats M&Ms and he is only appeased when the bag 

is empty'.) Every time I have indulged, however, I have always 

concluded “It wasn’t worth it” when I saw my high blood sugars 

or had to compensate for my over-estimation of my insulin. 


I also decided that I really, really like dark chocolate and 

given that it is full of antioxidants, I do allow myself some. I 

save my carbs for it. The 85% variety I eat has 4 g/square and 

is much more satisfying than milk chocolate, so it’s possible to 

enjoy without over-indulging. There are days too when I decide 

that it’s a “Garb Day.” For example, one very' hot day in the 

summer warrants my once-a-year small ice cream or custard 

cone. Or if some kind person makes me a birthday cake I’ll eat a 

bit. I call these excursions “experimental error.”I am not perfect, 



166 



Nutrition in Crisis 



but I try to keep my carbohydrate intake under 50 g/day and 

ideally around 30 g/day. For me, the lower my carbohydrate, the 

far better my control. 


Wendy Knapp Pogozelski earned a BS from Chatham University and 

a PhD in chemistry from Johns Hopkins University under the direction 

of Thomas Tullius She spent two years as an Office of Naval Research 

postdoctoral fellow working at various sites in radiation biology. She is a 

professor of chemistry at the State University of New York [SUNY) College 

at Geneseo, where she has been since 1996 She teaches biochemistry, 

emphasizing medical and nutrition-based applications. Her current 

research focuses on radiation effects on mitochondrial function and 

mitochondrial DNA as well as on understanding how dietary strategies 

affect biochemical pathways 




-CHAPTER 11- 



Metabolic Syndrome 


The Big Pitch 



T hrough the last two chapters, we’ve learned that diabetes repre¬ 

sents the most clear-cut example of how the glucose-insulin axis 

affects health. Diabetes rests at the center of nutritional thinking, 

whether or not you are a patient yourself. If you are primarily interested in 

weight loss or cardiovascular disease (CVD)—or even if your concern is 

general good health—insulin metabolism is always in the foreground. The 

focal point of so many of the issues we’ve discussed is metabolic syndrome 

(MetS). The idea is generally credited to Gerald Reaven, an endocrinolo¬ 

gist who died in 2018 at age eighty-nine. His original observation, which 

sparked the idea of MetS, was that overweight, high blood glucose, high 

blood pressure, and the lipid markers assumed to indicate cardiovascu¬ 

lar risk commonly appeared together in the same patients (table 11.1). 1 

MetS is not a disease but rather a collection of physiological markers that 

represent risk of disease. The separate components of the syndrome are not 

just superficially related, either. Reaven’s insight was to suggest that the 

markers arose from some common central cause, which he, and most of us, 

see as disruption in the glucose-insulin axis. 


The concept has now been extended to include several other physiologic 

markers, including inflammation and LDL particle size, that also seem 

to be tied together. Insulin is credited with control of the syndrome, and 

fittingly, several years before his death, Reaven insisted that MetS should 

be called “insulin-resistance syndrome.” 2 


The importance of metabolic syndrome came through to me a few years 

ago. I was listening to a presentation at a seminar on metabolic syndrome 

and its underlying cell biology. I don’t remember the details of the presen¬ 

tation, but I thought it was quite good. The speaker was also a doctor—in 



168 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Table 11.1. National Cholesterol Education Program Adult Treatment Panel III Definition 

of Metabolic Syndrome-Subjects have three of the following criteria 



Men Women 



Abdominal obesity, 

waist circumference 


> 40 inches 


> 35 inches 


Hypertriglyceridemia 


> 150 mg/dL 


>150 mg/dL 


Low HDL-cholesterol 


< 40 mg/dL 


< 50 mg/dL 


High blood pressure 


> 130/85 mm Hg 


> 130/85 mm Hg 


High fasting glucose 


> 110 mg/dL 


> 110 mg/dL 



Source NCEP Expert Panel on Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood 

Cholesterol in Adults (Bethesda, MD National Institutes of Health, 2001] 



fact, he was Mike Huckabee’s doctor. After the seminar, I asked him about 

low-carbohydrate diets and he unexpectedly went ballistic. “Go to the 

Atkins website. You can eat all the bacon you want. That's what it says.” 

I was somewhat taken aback. Did I say Atkins? I didn't really know what 

to say. I noticed that he still had on the screen his last slide showing the 

criteria for metabolic syndrome (something like table 11.1). 


Pointing to the screen, I said, “You know, all of those markers are exactly 

the things that are improved by low-carb diets.” He said, “Well, they're also 

improved by low-calorie diets,” which is simply not true. Low-fat diets, or at 

least high-carbohydrate diets, will not improve triglycerides and other mark¬ 

ers, and will more likely make them worse. Plus the level of blood glucose is 

determined by carbohydrate in the diet. Everybody agrees on that. 


The incident stuck in my head. A little later that day it occurred to me 

that I might have said something smart. The features of MetS were known 

to be improved by carbohydrate restriction, but this was not generally stated 

explicitly, or maybe its full import was not realized. Turning it around in 

your mind, you might say that the response to a low-carbohydrate diet 

could actually be the essential feature of metabolic syndrome, a kind of 

operational definition. 


The existence of a syndrome—that is, the simultaneous appearance of 

the markers—is no longer in question. It is also widely accepted that the 

syndrome might be a reflection of insulin resistance. Yet the full impact of 

MetS has not yet been appreciated. Its clinical importance continues to be 




















Metabolic Syndrome 



169 



questioned by some critics who claim that MetS is not a useful idea. These 

critics hold that saying your patient has the markers of MetS provides no 

more information than the primary observation that the markers do in fact 

often show up together. What they mean is that, whatever the underlying 

causes, the only way to treat markers A, B, and C (for example, obesity, 

diabetes, and high cholesterol) is with a drug for A, a drug for B, and a 

drug for C. In other words, they don’t have a single drug for MetS—only 

for each of the individual markers. 


The fact that A, B, and C can all be treated with a single interven¬ 

tion, namely, a low-carbohydrate diet, suggests that all the markers do in 

fact arise from a common cause: a disruption in the glucose—insulin axis, 

roughly described as insulin resistance. Low-carbohydrate diets could 

thus provide a working definition of the syndrome. If your patient had 

the markers traditionally associated with MetS and they improved with 

a low-carbohydrate diet, it would confirm that the patient did indeed 

have metabolic syndrome. I knew this was a good idea because other 

people had brought it up before. I had even written a paper myself titled 

“Metabolic Syndrome and Low-Carbohydrate Ketogenic Diets in the 

Medical School Biochemistry Curriculum,” but I hadn’t really seen the 

impact. There is a step in the evolution of ideas when you realize that a 

comment you made in passing has important implications and has to be 

restated as a law. 


If all the markers of metabolic syndrome could be improved by a low- 

carbohydrate diet, possibly even in the absence of calorie restriction, than 

what would that mean for the millions of people facing the risk predicted 

by these markers? What would it mean for the drugs that treat each indi¬ 

vidual condition but ignore the root cause? Lastly, what would it mean for 

those national authorities on health that were, and are still, recommend¬ 

ing a low-fat, high-carbohydrate diet? If the experimental data are there, 

would the nutritional establishment embrace them, even though they 

contain the words “low-carbohydrate”? It was 2005 and we really thought 

that progress could be made. Talk about the naivete of youth. 


Jeff Volek and I went through the literature and tabulated the responses 

to low-carbohydrate and low-fat diets with respect to the markers of 

MetS. The results, which we published in Nutrition and Metabolism? were 

as we expected (figure 11.1). I was the editor of Nutrition and Metabolism 




170 



Nutrition in Crisis 



at the time and thought that the paper would improve the standing of 

our journal. We recognized that we were dealing with modern science, 

where people don’t even have time to read an abstract, so we put the 

whole story in the title: “Carbohydrate Restriction Improves the Features 

of Metabolic Syndrome. Metabolic Syndrome May Be Defined by the 

Response to Carbohydrate Restriction ”The data showed that except for a 

couple of measurements (insulin, fasting glucose), the markers of MetS are 

improved by a low-carbohydrate diet, sometimes dramatically. Probably 

the best indicator of CVD risk based on commonly measured parameters 

is the ratio of triglycerides :HDL where the reduction is typically three to 

ten times greater in carbohydrate reduction. 



Volek’s Test of the Theory 


In chapter 7, we looked at the experiments in Jeff Volek’s laboratory show¬ 

ing that saturated fat in the blood was reduced by a low-carbohydrate diet 

with high saturated fat, as compared to low-fat diet with low saturated 

fat. The results on control of plasma saturated fat are critical because the 

presence of dietary saturated fat is still held up as an objection against 

low-carbohydrate diets. It was also the magnitude of the effect in Voleks 

experiment that was surprising. The total SFA fraction in the low-carbo- 

hydrate group was reduced by more than half, and this reduction was more 

than three times the average change in the low-fat group. 


This study had a larger overall significance, however. The real power 

of Voleks experiments was that the participants all fit the definition of 

metabolic syndrome, and that a wide variety of lipid and physiologic 

parameters were measured. Figure 11.1 shows that everything got better: 

HDL, insulin, leptin, and most dramatically, triglycerides. 



The Pitch 


This chapter reinforces the big pitch: If the coincidence of metabolic 

markers that defines MetS indicates a unified mechanism, if seemingly 

different physiologic effects—overweight, high blood pressure, athero¬ 

genic dyslipidemia, high triglycerides, low HDL, high blood glucose, high 

insulin—are all a reflection of a common underlying stimulus (proposed to 



Metabolic Syndrome 



171 



Body Abdominal 

Mass Fat TG 



ApoB/ 


Small 


Total 


TG/HDL ApoA-1 


LDL Glucose Insulin 


SFA 




Figure 11.1. Summary of responses of forty people with metabolic syndrome to a very 

low-carbohydrate ketogenic diet [VLCKD] or a low-fat diet (LFD). Data from J. S. Volek 

et al., "Carbohydrate Restriction Has a More Favorable Impact on the Metabolic Syndrome 

than a Low Fat Diet" Lipids 44, no. 4 (2009]: 297-309. 



be disruption in insulin metabolism), then if we can treat any one of those 

features, we can treat them all. 


Nothing is better than low-carbohydrate for weight loss, but other 

diets do work. Its harder for women and its harder as you get older, but 

there are lots of ways to get thinner. We don’t really have the answer on 

C VD. We know a lot of things that might be relevant but we don’t really 

know the fundamental cause. However, what we do know with some 

certainty is about diabetes: Cutting back on carbohydrate is the most 

effective treatment. For many it is a virtual cure. In the long term, it is 

better than drugs. So if MetS is really a clue to an underlying mechanism 

for all of the disparate markers, then effectively treating hyperglycemia 

will improve all of the features of MetS—that is to say, we have a 

prescription for general health. 




172 



Nutrition in Crisis 



The Head-and-Shoulders Effect 


In addition to metabolic effects, a low-carbohydrate diet -will 

cure irritable bowel syndrome and related disorders in many 

people. (Cancer is waiting in the wings, too.) In some sense, the 

problem with convincing people of the benefits of a reduced 

carbohydrate strategy is that it appears to be good for every¬ 

thing, good for what ails you. You can sound like a hard-sell 

pitchman. I call this the Head-and-Shoulders effect. I don’t 

know whether it is true but a rep from Procter 6c Gamble once 

told me that when they first brought out the shampoo of that 

name, they advertised that it would cure your dandruff in three 

days. What their tests actually showed was that it would cure 

it in one day, but they didn’t think anybody would believe that. 

It is probably not that low-carbohydrate is so good but that 

high-carbohydrate is so bad. 



PART 4 


The Mess in 

Nutritional Science 




-CHAPTER 12- 



The Medical Literature 


A Guide to Flawed Studies 



N utrition is in crisis. Almost every day a new study shows that 

you are at risk for diabetes, cardiovascular disease, or all-cause 

mortality brought on by a newly identified toxin that turns out 

to be something that you just had for lunch. It is not clear that any of these 

studies are subject to serious critical peer review, and for the curious, blog¬ 

gers usually do a good job of dismembering them. The continuous cycle 

of weak studies and their deconstruction goes beyond mere time wasting. 

People are hurt because bad recommendations are left out there, even when 

research shows that they are inappropriate and, in the process, science 

takes a big hit. The editors and reviewers of technical and medical journals 

who conduct peer review are supposed to act as the gatekeepers of scientific 

evidence, but the journals continue to publish papers showing very weak 

associations, and even some that are grossly misleading or contradictory. 

The media, which might be expected to help our case, make it worse. It’s 

not really their fault. A science reporter cannot reasonably have the time to 

read the original study in detail, and must instead accept the conclusions 

in the abstract, and so that message is transmitted through mass media. 

When you do explain to the reporter how misleading these reports are, 

and how people will be hurt, they are truly concerned and sympathetic, 

but they don’t always have complete editorial control. In any case they 

would like to help, but tomorrow they have to cover a story that may be 

even worse. It is really hard for the consumer. This post from Facebook 

probably tells the story: “So epically confused about diet. Everything I 

read is contradictory on epic proportions. About the only consistencies are 

low-sugar raw veggies and water. How in the world is a girl to sort it out, 

other than try everything and see what works for me?” 



176 



Nutrition in Crisis 



She went on to ask why it isn’t “possible to come up with a system 

that takes inputs—body stats and genetic history—and outputs a general 

reasonable diet to follow?” Chances are that the population at large is no 

more comfortable than this woman on Facebook when it comes to matters 

of nutrition. I wrote to her through private messages and reiterated the first 

three rules: (1) If you are okay, you are okay; (2) if you want to lose weight, 

don’t eat, and if you have to eat, don’t eat carbs; and (3) if you have diabetes 

or metabolic syndrome, you have to try a low-carbohydrate diet first. 


It is likely that many people wind up believing nothing at all, however, 

and simply assume that everything is exaggerated, save for the most 

ingrained popular notion that “maybe fat is bad and maybe I should not 

put so much salt on my food.” Then there is the progression of articles on 

raspberry-ketones, resveratrol, trans-fats, and methylglyoxal, each of which 

will either kill you or save you, depending on whom you ask. 


Most discouraging are the health agencies. The American Diabetes 

Association (ADA) wants people with diabetes to consume a lot of carbo¬ 

hydrates. They keep saying that they don’t have a diet, and that they’re 

not opposed to low-carbohydrate diets (for weight loss)—but instead they 

stress “individualization” without any indication as to which individuals 

benefit from which intervention. Despite the disclaimers and ever-shifting 

language, there is no doubt that the ADA is perceived as opposing low- 

carbohydrate—and it seems clear they are the only ones responsible for 

that perception. 


The evidence that weight loss is not required for improvement in diabe¬ 

tes, from the work of Nuttall and Gannon, 1 for example, is not mentioned 

by the ADA. They know about that evidence. I’m sure of it because I have 

told members of the committee personally, and they should already be 

aware because some of the work was funded by the ADA itself. The ADA 

guidelines do not cite important scientific work showing that weight loss 

is not required for improvement in diabetes. People who are not scientists 

ask me, understandably incredulous, “Can you do that? Are you allowed to 

make recommendations without citing other people’s work?” 


There is a daily progression of sweeping statements that go way beyond 

the published data. At the same time there remains an inability or an unwill¬ 

ingness to zero in on real factors. The low-carbohydrate diet has attained 

the status of the name of God in Hebrew: It must never be said out loud. 



The Medical Literature 



177 



Part of the problem is that the literature, especially major medical 

publications, is still predominantly subscription-based. Most people 

cannot access the information, so the results are then fed downstream to 

the media, who take anything they are fed at face value, and then pass it on 

further to the general public. 


The rigid dogma of the literature has reached Galilean proportions. 

Fructose and sugar are bad (unless you try to lump them in with all carbo¬ 

hydrates). If you want your paper on fructose to be published, begin with: 

“Because of the deleterious effects of dietary fructose, we hypothesized 

that . . .” Never start with: “Whether dietary fructose has a deleterious 

effect. . .” Our paper on fructose was published with “whether ..as the 

opening sentence, 2 but only after a hard-fought rebuttal of reviewers’ criti¬ 

cisms that turned out to be fifteen pages long. Worse, if you even mention 

low-carbohydrate, you are guaranteed real grief. When the Journal of 

the American Medical Association published George Bray’s “calorie-is-a- 

calorie” 3 and I pointed out that the study more accurately supported the 

importance of carbohydrate as a controlling variable, the editor refused to 

publish my letter. Thankfully, blogs have performed a valuable service by 

providing an alternative point of view, but if unreliability is a problem in 

the scientific literature, that problem is multiplied exponentially in internet 

sources. In the end, consumers might feel that they are pretty much on 

their own. 


It does take some confidence, especially for the layperson, to feel 

that their intuitive understanding is correct: that the difference between 

white rice and brown rice is so small that it really doesn’t matter what 

Harvard’s computer says. Most researchers are very much disinclined to 

get into a shooting match, or worse, whistle-blowing. The long blue line 

is a strong force in repressing investigation, not because the authorities 

think corruption is okay, but because scandal reflects badly on everybody. 

Whistle-blowing in this field is especially weird because sometimes the 

transgressions are right out in the open. 



Statistics: Death of the Medical Literature 


Many scientists believe that if you do a good experiment, you don’t need 

statistics. David Colquohon, a well-known neuroscientist and critic of 



178 



Nutrition in Crisis 



The Golden Rule of Statistics 


Here’s the Golden Rule for reading a scientific paper, from the 

book PDQ Statistics by Norman and Streiner: “The important 

point ... is that the onus is on the author to convey to the 

reader an accurate impression of what the data look like, using 

graphs or standard measures, before beginning the statistical 

shenanigans. Any paper that doesn't do this should be viewed 

from the outset with considerable suspicion.” 4 


In other words, explain things clearly to the reader. There are 

complicated ideas in science but the often quoted statement 

(only once before in this book) from Einstein, that you should 

make it simple but not too simple, is a reasonable demand for 

you, the reader, to make of the scientific literature. 



poor scientific method, agrees. Colquohon is the author of an excellent, 

if technical, statistics book, which is now freely available online. 5 The 

introduction to his book points out: “The snag, of course, is that doing 

good experiments is difficult. Most people need all the help they can get 

to prevent them making fools of themselves by claiming that their favorite 

theory is substantiated by observations that do nothing of the sort.” 


This kind of circumspection is, unfortunately, more common among 

people who write the statistics books than those who use them. A good 

statistics book will have an introduction that says something like “In 

statistics, we try to put a number on our intuition.” In other words, it 

is not really, by itself, a science. It is, or should be, a tool for the experi¬ 

menter's use, and like any tool, you have to know how to use it. Because 

statistics offers so many tools, there is not always agreement on which 

hammer goes with which nail. All of statistics is interpretation. The 

problem is that many authors of papers in the medical literature allow 

statistics to become their master rather than their servant: -Numbers 

are plugged into a statistical program and the results are interpreted in 




The Medical Literature 



179 



a cut-and-dried fashion. Statistical significance (that two sets of data 

are not from the same population) is confused with clinical significance 

(that differences are sufficiently large to have a biological effect). Misuse 

of statistics is the subject of numerous papers and books, but this has 

had little effect. 



Statistical Shenanigans 

versus Common Sense 


How do you deal with the reports in the press that tell you that white 

rice will give you diabetes but brown rice wont? The saving principle is 

that, despite its subtleties and reliance on mathematics, biochemistry 

uses the same basic rules of logic as daily life. So the first question you 

have to ask is whether a given study makes sense: How is it possible 

that white rice is so different from brown rice? A moment s thought 

suggests that the major difference between the two is in the stuff that 

isn’t even digested, the fiber. The nutritional establishment is always 

pushing fiber—whole grain and all that—so you might want to hear 

their case, but then there is the possibility that the entire fiber thing 

itself is questionable or at least exaggerated—and what of the Asian 

societies that are always invoked to tell us how much we need grain, yet 

never ate brown rice. Never. So, it can be confusing, and common sense 

makes you suspicious. 



Habeas Corpus Datorum 


Science is an extension of common sense, but that doesnt mean there 

aren’t revolutionary ideas. You want to be suspicious of a revolutionary 

idea if it violates common sense, but you can’t throw out the idea just for 

that reason. The solution is simple, if not always easy to implement. If it is 

a reasonable conclusion, you can cut the author some slack. If the idea is 

far out, you need to see the data—all the data—not just the average of the 

data or conclusions from the computer. My new grand principle of doing 

science: habeas corpus datorum. Let’s see the body of the data. If the conclu¬ 

sion is nonintuitive and goes against previous work or common sense, then 

the data must be strong and clearly presented. 



180 



Nutrition in Crisis 



So, how should you read a scientific paper? I usually want to see the 

pictures, or figures, first. It s not just about saving a thousand words, as the 

saying goes. Its about presentation of the data, and its about the Golden Rule 

of Statistics: “Convey to the reader an accurate impression of what the data 

look like, using graphs or standard measures, before beginning the statistical 

shenanigans.” On the topic of tables: Figures are so much better than tables 

that a whole book, Medical Illuminations , was written about the idea. 6 


Many of us write scientific papers in the same way we read them. We 

make the figures first and then try to figure out what they say. The prin¬ 

ciple: A scientific paper is supposed to explain. I tell graduate students 

that if you do an experiment and you don’t explain it well, it’s as if you 

never did it at all. In teaching students how to present their work, I ask 

them: “Describe what you are supposed to do in a scientific seminar or 

other presentation.” Sometimes they begin to say something, but I usually 

make it worse by adding: “No. In one word. What are you supposed to 

do? One word.” Having reached an appropriate level of annoyance where 

they will be relieved to hear the answer, I give it to them: “Teach.” You 

want to explain things to your audience. The same is true of a scientific 

paper. Again, the Golden Rule of Statistics: The onus is on the author. 



Caveat Lector 


Presenting a scientific paper is also a bit like selling something. Scientific 

papers are rarely just data. They have an idea that they are trying to sell. 

Teaching and selling are the two things you do in science. The reader has 

to be an educated consumer, however, and as an educated consumer she or 

he should be suspicious of overselling. One good indicator of overselling 

is the use of value judgments as if they were scientific terms. “Healthy” (or 

“healthful”) is not a scientific term. 


If a study describes a diet as “healthy,” it is almost guaranteed to be a 

flawed study. If we knew which diets were “healthy,” we wouldn’t have an 

obesity epidemic. A good example is the paper by Appel on the DASH 

diet, which concluded: “In the setting of a healthful diet, partial substi¬ 

tution of carbohydrate with either protein or monounsaturated fat can 

further lower blood pressure, improve lipid levels, and reduce estimated 

cardiovascular risk.” 7 



The Medical Literature 



181 



It’s hard to know how healthful the original diet could have been if 

removing carbohydrate improved everything. In addition, not only was 

this about a “carbohydrate-rich diet used in the DASH trials” but it is 

“currently advocated in several scientific reports.” Another red flag is when 

the authors tell you how widely accepted their idea is. 


Understatement is good. One of the more famous is from the classic 

Watson and Crick paper of 1953, in which the two researchers proposed 

the DNA double helix structure. They said, “It has not escaped our notice 

that the specific pairing we have postulated immediately suggests a possible 

copying mechanism for the genetic material.” 


What Is Wrong with the Literature 

and What Is Right 


Let’s start with what’s right. There are many published papers in the 

nutritional literature that are informative, creative, and generally conform 

to the standards of good science. Naturally, like any scientific literature, 

most papers are fairly routine. They are of specialized interest, or as 

described in one of the choices on the checklist for referees who review 

manuscripts—“of interest to other workers in the field.” 


What’s wrong is not the mediocre papers but rather the surprising number 

of really objectionable papers. The medical literature is full of papers border¬ 

ing on fraud, or at least, guilty of misrepresentation. There are many papers 

that are full of fundamental errors and total lack of judgment in interpreta¬ 

tion. Worse, there is a possibility that somebody is going to get hurt because 

of bad medical advice that follows from these misinterpretations. 


Most work in most fields, more or less by definition, is mediocre. What 

makes papers in medical nutrition different is their drastic claims about 

saving hundreds of thousands of lives by scaling up a result that had hardly 

any effect to begin with to the entire population. 


It is difficult to face the fact that so much of the medical literature is 

published by people who are not trained in science, which means they 

don’t know the game, they haven’t seen much of it, and they wouldn’t know 

good science if they saw it. There is no real conceptual training in science. 

You can learn techniques, but it’s not about cyclotrons, it s about ideas. 

There is no reason why a physician can’t do real scientific thinking, but at 



182 



Nutrition in Crisis 



the same time, there is no reason why he or she can. An MD degree is not 

a guarantee of any expertise outside the practitioner’s area of specialty. 


The irony is that the practice of medicine can be highly scientific. 

Differential diagnosis and the experience in recommending the right drug 

are the kinds of things that are part of scientific disciplines. The same 

physician who will intuitively solve a medical mystery, though, will assume 

that, for scientific research, things are different—that there are somehow 

arbitrary rules and that brute-force application of statistics will tell you 

whether what you did is true. 


The next six chapters will detail the failures of nutritional literature, 

ranging from the slightly inaccurate—“association does not imply causal¬ 

ity” (sometimes it does and sometimes it doesn’t)-—to the idiotic—“you 

must do intention-to-treat” (if you assign subjects to take a drug and they 

don’t take it, you have to include their data with those who did). I will also 

cover “levels of evidence”: arbitrary rules that get incorporated into tables, 

the top of which is always some kind of “gold standard.” The odd thing 

about levels of scientific evidence is that nobody in any physical science 

would recognize them. They are, in fact, the creation of people who are 

trying to do science but wouldn’t know science if they saw it—fundamen¬ 

tally amateurs who have arbitrary rules along the lines of the apocryphal 

story about Mozart: 


A man comes to Mozart and wants to become a composer. 

Mozart says that he has to study theory for a couple of years, 

that he should study orchestration and become proficient at the 

piano, and goes on like this. Finally the man says, “But you 

wrote your first symphony when you were eight years old.” 

Mozart says, “Yes, but I didn’t ask anybody.” 



The Bottom Line 


To understand research in nutrition, or really, any science, one has to be 

prepared to question “the experts.” If a paper does not adhere to the Golden 

Rule and is too quick to begin “the statistical shenanigans,” it “should be 

viewed from the outset with considerable suspicion.” 



The Medical Literature 



183 



The bottom line is that you have to expect real communication from 

the authors of a scientific paper. The problem, for many people, lies in the 

difficulty of believing that the best and the brightest are at fault. But it is 

not hard to find examples of experts making mistakes. I will try to provide 

you with all the help I can in the following chapters. 



-CHAPTER 13- 



Observational Studies, 

Association, Causality 



789 deaths were reported in Doll and Hills original cohort. 

Thirty-six of these were attributed to lung cancer. When 

these lung cancer deaths were counted in smokers versus non- 

smokers, the correlation virtually sprang out: all thirty-six of 

the deaths had occurred in smokers. The difference between the 

two groups was so significant that Doll and Hill did not even 

need to apply complex statistical metrics to discern it. The trial 

designed to bring the most rigorous statistical analysis to the 

cause of lung cancer barely required elementary mathematics 



to prove his point. 



—SlDDHARTHA MuKHERJEE, 


The Emperor of All Maladies 



S cientists don’t like philosophy of science. It is not just that pomp 

ous phrases like hypothetico-deductive systems are such a turnoff; 

it’s that we rarely recognize descriptions of science in philosophy 

articles as accurate reflections of what we actually do. In the end, there is 

no definition of science any more than there is a definition for music or for 

literature. Because scientists have different styles, it is hard to generalize 

about actual scientific behavior. Research is a human activity, and precisely 

because it puts a premium on creativity, it defies categorization. As the 

physicist Steven Weinberg put it, echoing Justice Stewart on pornography: 

“There is no logical formula that establishes a sharp dividing line between 

a beautiful explanatory theory and a mere list of data, but we know the 

difference when we see it—we demand a simplicity and rigidity in our 

principles before we are willing to take them seriously. 1 



186 



Nutrition in Crisis 



We know, however, that what we see in the current state of nutrition is 

not “a beautiful explanatory theory” or anything of the sort. This forces us 

to consider what it is that makes nutritional medical literature so bad. If 

we can identify some principles, maybe we can penetrate the mess and see 

how it could be fixed. 


One frequently stated principle is that “observational studies only 

generate hypotheses.” There is the related principle that “association does 

not imply causality,” usually cited in a backhanded way by those authors 

who want you to believe that the association they found does in fact imply 

causality. These two principles are not exactly right. They fail to recognize 

that scientific experiments are not so easily wedged into categories like 

“observational studies.” Principles like “observational studies only generate 

hypotheses” are also widely invoked by bloggers and critics to discredit 

the continuing stream of observational studies that make an association 

between their favored targets—eggs, red meat, sugar-sweetened soda—and 

prevalence of some metabolic disease or cancer. In most cases, the original 

studies are getting what they deserve, but the bills of indictment are not 

accurate, and it would be better not to cite absolute statements of scientific 

principles. It is not simply that these studies are observational studies, but 

rather that they are bad observational studies, and in many cases, the asso¬ 

ciations that they find are so weak that the study—if anything—constitutes 

an argument against causality. On the assumption that good experimental 

practice and interpretation could be roughly defined, I laid out a few prin¬ 

ciples that I thought were a better representation—if you can even make 

such generalization—of what actually goes on in science: 


* Observations generate hypotheses. Observational studies test hypoth¬ 

eses. Associations do not necessarily imply causality. In some sense, all 

science is associations. 


* Only mathematics is axiomatic (starts from absolute assumptions). 


* If you notice that kids who eat a lot of candy seem to be fat, or even if you 

notice that you yourself get fat eating candy, that is an observation. From 

this observation, you might come up with the hypothesis that sugar 

causes obesity. Thus, an observation generates hypotheses. A test of your 

hypothesis would be to carry out an observational study. For example, you 

might try to see if there is an association between sugar consumption and 



Observational Studies, Association, Causality 187 



incidence of obesity. There are different ways of doing this—the simplest 

epidemiologic approach is simply to compare the history of the eating 

behavior of individuals (insofar as you can get it) with how fat they are. 

When you do this comparison you are testing your hypothesis. 


For the final point on the list, you must remember that there are an 

infinite number of other things—meat consumption, TV hours, distance 

from a French bakery, grandfather’s waist circumference—that you could 

have measured as an independent variable. Your hypothesis, however, is 

that the variable is candy. What about all the others? Mike Eades, author 

of the influential Protein Power, described falling asleep as a child by trying 

to think of everything in the world. You just can’t test them all. As Einstein 

put it, “Your theory determines the measurement you make.” If you found 

associations with everything, would anything be causal? 



Association Can Predict Causality 


In fact, association can provide strong evidence for causation. If we didnt ask 

about the extent to which the tacos we ate were the cause of our stomach 

upset, we would not function well in our daily lives. Single observations might 

generate a hypothesis or a guess that we can test. We can determine, for 

example, whether a particular restaurant is good through continuous “test¬ 

ing.” Single observations generate hypotheses. Hypotheses, in turn, generate 

observational studies, not the other way around. A correct statement is that 

association does not necessarily imply causation. In some sense, all science is 

observation and association. Even thermodynamics, the most mathematical 

and absolute of sciences, rests on observation. We have never observed a true 

perpetual motion machine, so we have generalized that observation into a 

physical law, but as soon as somebody makes one that works, it s all over. 


Biological mechanisms, or perhaps all scientific theories, are never 

proved. By legal analogy: You cannot be found innocent, only not guilty. 

That is why excluding a theory is stronger than showing consistency. The 

grand epidemiological study of macronutrient intake the association of 

what Americans ate in the last forty years and the incidence of diabetes and 

obesity—shows that increased carbohydrate is associated with increased 

calories, even under conditions where fruits and vegetables also go up 



188 



Nutrition in Crisis 



and where fat, if anything, goes down. The data on dietary consumption 

and disease in the whole population can be described as an observational 

study, but it is strong because it gives support to a lack of causal effect of 

decreased fat on positive outcome—it excludes a theory 


Again, in science, finding a contradiction has greater impact than 

merely finding a consistent result. The multitude of prospective experi¬ 

ments (where you pick the population first and see how people do on your 

variable of interest) have shown in the past, and will undoubtedly continue 

to show, the same lack of relation between fat intake and disease. But will 

anybody give up on saturated fat? In a court of law, if you are found not 

guilty of child abuse, people may still not let you move into their neighbor¬ 

hood. An association will tell you about causality if: (1) the association is 

strong; (2) there is a plausible underlying mechanism; and (3) there is not 

a more plausible explanation. 


The often-cited coincidental correlation between cardiovascular disease 

and number of TV sets does not imply causality because, although the 

first principle of observational studies is observed, there is no logical direct 

underlying mechanism. TV does not cause CVD. Interestingly, in the case 

of CVD, where so many associations have been published, and so many 

learned societies have told us how to prevent or cure the disease, there 

remains little in the way of an agreed upon mechanism. 


Re-Inventing the Wheel: Me and Bradford Hill 


This chapter is a reworking of a blog post that I published in 2013. The post 

included the principles I laid out at the top of the chapter for dealing with 

the kind of observational studies that you see in the scientific literature. I 

was speaking off the top of my head, trying to describe the logic that scien¬ 

tists use in interpreting data. It was an obvious description of what is done 

in practice. I didn’t think it was particularly original, and again, I don’t think 

that there are any hard and fast principles in science. When I described 

what I had written to my colleague, Dr. Eugene Fine, his response was, 

“Aren’t you re-inventing the wheel?” He meant that Bradford Hill, pretty 

much the inventor of modern epidemiology, had already established these 

and a couple of others as principles. In our conversation, Eugene cited The 

Emperor of All Maladies? an outstanding book on the history of cancer. I 



Observational Studies, Association, Causality 189 



had, in fact, read Emperor on his recommendation. I remembered Bradford 

Hill and the description of the evolution of the ideas of epidemiology, 

population studies, and randomized controlled trials. The story is also told 

in James LeFanu’s The Rise and Fall of Modern Medicine , 3 another captivat¬ 

ing history of medicine. 


I thought of these as general philosophical ideas, rather than as absolute 

scientific principles. Perhaps it is that we’re just used to it, but saying that 

an association has to be very strong to imply causality is common sense, 

and not in the same ballpark with the Pythagorean theorem. It’s some¬ 

thing that you might say over coffee or in response to somebody’s blog. 

Being explicit about it turns out to be very important, but like much in 

philosophy of science, it struck me as not of great intellectual import. It all 

reminded me of learning, in grade school, that the Earl of Sandwich had 

invented the sandwich. At which time I thought, “This is an invention? 

Woody Allen thought the same thing years later and wrote the history 

of the sandwich. He recorded the Earl’s early failures: “In 1741, he places 

bread on bread with turkey on top. This fails. In 1745, he exhibits bread 

with turkey on either side. Everyone rejects this except David Hume.” 


In fact, Hill’s principles remain important even if seemingly obvious. The 

pervasive violation of these principles in the medical literature constitutes 

their real significance. The concept of the randomized controlled trial 

(RCT)—randomly assigning people to a drug or behavior that you’re test¬ 

ing, or to a group that is the control—while obvious to us now, was hard 

won. Likewise, proving that any particular environmental factor—diet, 

smoking, pollution, or toxic chemicals—was the cause of a disease, and that 

by reducing that factor, the disease could be prevented, turned out to be a 

very hard sell, especially to physicians whose view of disease might have 

been strongly colored by the idea of an infective agent, bacterium, or virus. 


The Rise and Fall of Modern Medicine describes Bradford Hill’s two 

important contributions 4 : He demonstrated that tuberculosis could 

be cured by a combination of two drugs, streptomycin and PAS (para- 

aminosalicylic acid), and even more importantly, he showed that tobacco 

causes lung cancer. Hill was a professor of medical statistics at the London 

School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, but was not formally trained in 

statistics, and like many of us, thought of proper statistics simply as applied 

common sense. Ironically, an early near-fatal case of tuberculosis prevented 



190 



Nutrition in Crisis 



formal medical education. His first monumental accomplishment was, in 

fact, to demonstrate how tuberculosis was cured by the streptomycin-PAS 

combination. In 1941, Hill and his coworker Richard Doll undertook a 

systematic investigation of the risk factors for lung cancer. His eventual 

success was accompanied by a description of the principles that allow you 

to say when association can be taken as causation. 



Association and Causality: The Nine Criteria 


Bradford Hill described the factors that might lead you to believe that an 

association supports a causal role. Hill s criteria are still perfectly reasonable 

today, though in the current medical literature, they are probably much 

more widely practiced in the breach than the observance. The diligent 

application of Hills principles to the medical literature would substantially 

reduce the size of the literature and improve the quality of what remained: 


1. STRENGTH. “First upon my list I would put the strength of the 

association,” Hill wrote. This, of course, is exactly what is missing in the 

continued epidemiological scare stories whose measures of relative risk are 

so small. Hill describes: 


Prospective inquiries into smoking have shown that the death rate 

from cancer of the lung in cigarette smokers is nine to ten times 

the rate in non-smokers and the rate in heavy cigarette smokers 

is twenty to thirty times as great.... On the other hand the death 

rate from coronary thrombosis in smokers is no more than twice, 

possibly less, than the death rate in non-smokers. Though there is 

good evidence to support causation it is surely much easier in this 

case to think of some features of life that may go hand-in-hand 

with smoking—features that might conceivably be the real under¬ 

lying cause or, at the least, an important contributor, whether it be 

lack of exercise, nature of diet or other factors. 


Hill expressed doubts about a relative risk of 2 or less. Criticized else¬ 

where in this book, relative risk (RR) is what it sounds like: the ratio of the 

risks from two outcomes. Risk is the probability of an outcome; relative 

risk is the ratio of the individual risks of two events. For example, if you 



Observational Studies, Association, Causality 



191 



were to compare a group of factory workers in a chemical plant, say, to 

the general population and you found that for every 1,000 workers, 26 

developed cancer, then the probability, the risk of cancer is 26 out of 1,000 

or 0.026 or 2.6 percent. If you found in the general population that there 

were only 13 cases of cancer for every 1,000 people, then the risk for the 

general population is 13 out of 1,000 or 0.013 or 1.3 percent. You can then 

calculate relative risk as follows: 


RR = (risk for workers) / (risk for population) = (26/1,000) / (13/1,000)=2:1 


This might be considered evidence for environmental hazard, although as 

Hill says, it would be stronger if the RR were 10. Still 2 is considered grounds 

for taking the results seriously (or taking the factory owner to court). RR, 

however, as its name suggests, is relative. It hides information. The RR would 

still be 2 if there were 26 out of a million workers getting sick and 13 out of a 

million in the general population. The absolute risk is the difference between 

the probabilities. Absolute risk in this example is small (2.6 percent-1.3 

percent = 1.3 percent). In other words, not even a full 2 percent more factory 

workers got cancer than the general population. If you did take the factory 

owner to court, the judge might ask for additional evidence. 


This is what are we up against. Even within the limitations of rela¬ 

tive risk, the way in which the results are presented can significantly 

affect a reader's perception. The abstract from a study on eating breakfast 

found that: “Men who skipped breakfast had a 27% higher risk of CHD 

compared with men who did not.” 5 The number 27 percent sounds a great 

deal more impressive than the relative risk of 1.27, even though the latter 

might paint a better picture of the limited effects. For every 227 people 

with cancer, 100 will be regular breakfast eaters and 127 will have passed. 

By Hills standards, or by common sense, a relative risk of 1.27 would not 

make you force yourself to eat breakfast if you weren't hungry. Describing 

the results as “27% higher risk,” on the other hand, might influence your 

behavior more. Reporting relative risk so as to make the effect larger is the 

single most prevalent mistake in the medical literature and is even worse 

in the media that report on that literature. It must be noted, however, that 

while a high relative risk is no guarantee of causality, a low relative risk is 

definitely suspicious. 



192 



Nutrition in Crisis 



2. CONSISTENCY- Hill listed the repetition of the results of studies, 

under different circumstances, as a criterion for considering the extent to 

which an association implied causality. We expect results to be reproduc¬ 

ible, and a weak association might gain some strength if the observation is 

reproduced. Consistency of strong results, however, is what we want to see. 


Criterion 2 is not, however, independent of criterion 1. Although Hill 

himself did not mention this, it is of great importance. Consistently weak 

associations do not generally add up to a strong association. If there is a 

single practice in modern medicine that is completely out of whack with 

respect to careful consideration of causality, it is the use of meta-analyses 

where studies with marginal strengths are averaged to create a conclusion 

that is stronger than the majority of its components. In fact, many meta¬ 

analyses include studies that have not shown any association at all; the 

authors average them with a couple that have and then report the average 

as significant. Averaging studies without significant outcomes and expect¬ 

ing to get an effect is as foolish as it sounds, but it is widely practiced. 


3. SPECIFICITY. Hill was circumspect on this point, recognizing that 

we should have an open mind on what causes what. On specificity in the 

study of cancer and cigarettes, Hill "noted that the two sites where he had 

showed a cause-and-effect relationship were the lungs and the nose. 


4. TEMPORALITY- Obviously we expect the cause to precede the 

effect. Hill recognized that temporality was not so clear for diseases that 

developed slowly. “Does a particular diet lead to disease or do the early 

stages of the disease lead to those peculiar dietetic habits?” Of current 

interest are the epidemiologic studies that show a correlation between diet 

soda and obesity. These studies are quick to assert a causal link, but there is 

always a question of which way causation proceeds. One might reasonably 

ask, “What kind of people drink diet sodas?”The kind of people who want 

to lose weight. Most important from the temporal perspective, carbohy¬ 

drate restriction has immediate and dramatic effects on a disease, diabetes, 

that has severe acute symptoms. The fact that the “concerns” about long¬ 

term effects have never actually materialized pretty much defines the crisis 

in nutrition. 


5. BIOLOGICAL GRADIENT. Association should show a dose- 

response curve. In the case of cigarettes, the death rate from cancer of the 

lung increases linearly with the number of cigarettes smoked. A subset 



Observational Studies, Association, Causality 



193 



of the first principle—that the association should be strong—is that the 

dose-response curve should have a meaningful slope—that is, the differ¬ 

ence between the numbers at the beginning and the end of the scale 

should be substantial, and of course, should not show large fluctuations. 

Numerous studies in the literature can avoid putting their results to the 

test by lumping data into quartile. When fifty points are lumped into one 

quartile, it might show slope, but it would be nice to know the gradient 

within each quartile. 


6. PLAUSIBILITY. Hill said, “What is biologically plausible depends 

upon the biological knowledge of the day.” Here, Hills emphasis on effect 

size is important. If the association is far-fetched, unexpected, or derived 

from a less-than-obvious idea, there is a greater burden of proof and any 

association should be strong. 


7. COHERENCE. Data, according to Hill, “should not seriously conflict 

with the generally known facts of the natural history and biology of the 

disease.”The natural history of diabetes is the effect of carbohydrate—not 

fat, not red meat, not any protein. Any number of other factors might be 

involved, but if you dont put carbohydrate first, you are giving up on the 

common sense that Hill acknowledged was the basis of his criteria. 


8. EXPERIMENT. It was another age. It is hard to believe that it was 

in my lifetime that Hill proposed this principle: “Occasionally it is possible 

to appeal to experimental, or semi-experimental, evidence. For example, 

because of an observed association some preventive action is taken, does 

it, in fact, prevent?” The inventor of the randomized controlled trial would 

be amazed how many try to take preventative action, and how many, in 

fact, dont prevent—and most of all, he would have been astounded that it 

doesn’t seem to affect the opinion of the medical community. The progres¬ 

sion of failures, from the Framingham Study to the Womens Health 

Initiative, and the lack of association among low-fat, low saturated fat, and 

CVD, is strong evidence for the absence of causation. 


9. ANALOGY. “In some circumstances it would be fair to judge by 

analogy. With the effects of thalidomide and rubella before us, we would 

surely be ready to accept slighter but similar evidence with another drug or 

another viral disease in pregnancy.” 


With the list concluded, this is Hills final word on the criteria for 

determining causation: 



194 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Here then are nine different viewpoints from all of which we 

should study association before we cry causation. What I do not 

believe—and this has been suggested—is that we can usefully lay 

down some hard-and-fast rules of evidence that must be obeyed 

before we accept cause and effect. None of my nine viewpoints 

can bring indisputable evidence for or against the cause-and-effect 

hypothesis and none can be required as a sine qua non. What they 

can do, with greater or less strength, is to help us to make up our 

minds on the fundamental question— is there any other way of 

explaining the set of facts before us, is there any other answer equally, 

or more, likely than cause and effect? (Emphasis added) 


This might be the first critique of the still-to-be-invented evidence- 

based medicine. 



Nutritional Epidemiology 


There are many critics of current nutritional epidemiology and most of us 

don't understand how the field could persist with so many weak results. It 

is simply the low standards and the failure to understand that statistics is 

not data, and it is not a science as such. Statistics comes from a particular 

persons opinion on how the data should be interpreted. Conflicts then arise 

from differing opinions. You must adjudicate between two principles: one, 

“The risk is small but when you scale it up to the whole population, you will 

save thousands of lives”; and two, to which I am partial, “When risk is small, 

there is low predictability of outcome. You cant scale up bad data.” 


The real impact of Hill's criteria is that they provide standards for 

interpreting epidemiological studies. They are standards that still have a 

good deal of subjectivity, but they are standards nonetheless. Yet they are 

ignored in nutrition. They are ignored by authors, and most importantly, 

they are ignored by the reviewers and editors who are expected to be the 

gatekeepers on solid science. In the end, the decision that an observational 

study implies causation is another way of saying that it is meaningful— 

that it is not an outcome of mathematical juggling—that it is, you know, 

science. The Emperor of All Maladies describes Hills criteria as principles 

“which have remained in use by epidemiologists to date.” But have they? 



Observational Studies, Association, Causality 



195 



Many have voiced criticisms of epidemiology as its currently practiced in 

nutrition. One way to look at the current problems in nutrition is that we 

have a large number of research groups doing epidemiology in violation of 

Hill s criteria. 



Is It Science? 


Science is a human activity. What we dont like about philosophy of science 

is that it is about the structure of science, rather than about what scientists 

really do, and so there arent even any real definitions. Izja Lederhandler, a 

colleague at the NIH, put it well: “What you do in science is, you make a 

hypothesis and then you try to shoot yourself down.” A good experiment 

puts the experimenter s theory to the test. An experiment whose outcome 

only shows consistency is not strong. 


One of the most interesting insights on the work of Hill and Doll, as 

described in The Emperor of All Maladies, was that during breaks from the 

taxing work of analyzing the questionnaires on smoking, Doll himself 

would step out for a smoke. Doll believed that cigarettes were unlikely to 

be a cause—he favored tar from paved highways as the causative agent— 

but as the data came in, “in the middle of the survey, sufficiently alarmed, 

he gave up smoking.” 6 In science, you try to shoot yourself down and you 

go with the data. The mass pf papers demonizing fat, and the current flood 

of papers demonizing sugar, fail most of Hills criteria. A major reason 

that they fail is that they set out to show consistency between the data 

and the theory, rather than to challenge the theory. They dont try to shoot 

themselves down. As a result, they dont consider what other facts could 

equally explain the data. 


Navigating the Mess 


The goal of this chapter, and those that surround it, is to help the consumer— 

and perhaps other scientists—read scientific publications. There really are 

no set principles of science. The existence of a “gold standard”—that is, the 

one best type of experiment that answers all questions—is not recognized 

in any science. The best experiment is the one that answers the question at 

hand. Observational studies are appropriate if researchers cant intervene 



196 



Nutrition in Crisis 



for practical or ethical reasons. Such experiments imply causality if they 

show strong associations and if they have underlying mechanisms in basic 

chemistry or biology. 


Bradford Hill laid down principles for dealing with observational stud¬ 

ies, which he recognized as attempts to turn common sense into practice. 

Hill’s criteria, once again, are as follows: 



1. Strength 


2. Consistency 


3. Specificity 



4. Temporality 


5. Biological gradient 


6. Plausibility 



7. Coherence 


8. Experiment 


9. Analogy 



Hill showed a causal link between cigarettes and cancer because he 

found that deaths from lung cancer for cigarette smokers were nine to ten 

times that of nonsmokers, a ratio that went up to twenty for heavy smok¬ 

ers compared to nonsmokers. He was less sure about coronary thrombosis 

because the relative risk (RR) was in the range of 2:1. This standard is not 

even proposed in modern nutritional experimentation. A nearly continu¬ 

ous flow of papers in the medical literature showing that something that 

you eat will cause some disease you don’t want to have, even if the RR is as 

low as 1.3, has marred the field substantially. The majority of epidemiologic 

studies, in fact, violate Hill’s criteria. 


The most important feature of scientific behavior is, again, the mind-set. 

You have to have the courage to try to shoot your own theory down. This is 

consistent with the general principle that excluding a hypothesis is always 

stronger than showing consistency. The problem in nutrition is that experi¬ 

menters are trying to prove things, instead of trying to disprove things. 



-CHAPTER 14- 



Red Meat and 

the New Puritans 



Dost thou think, because thou art virtuous, there shall be no 

more cakes and ale ? 


—William Shakespeare,^^ 



E xperts on nutrition are like experts on sexuality. No matter how 

professional they might be, in some way, they are always trying to 

justify their own lifestyle. Sure that others should follow their own 

standards of behavior, they are always tempted to save us from our own sins, 

whether sexual or dietary. While our own New England Puritans were not 

actually down on sex (as described in Sara VowelTs Wordy Shipmates, they 

actually thought sex was an argument for Gods existence), they did have 

the obsessive and literal insistence on what God really wanted. Sin was on 

their minds. 


The new Puritans of contemporary America want to save us from red 

meat. It is unknown whether Michael Pollans In Defense of Food 1 was 

reporting the news or making it, but Pollan’s recommendation to limit 

meat consumption has become commonplace. Vegetarian Times says that 

3.2 percent of the US population is vegetarian, and the Harvard School of 

Public Health thinks that they are the righteous few among us. 


There are good reasons for avoiding meat—better not to kill 

anything, generally—but for most people, the health angle is not one 

of the reasons. We are also suspicious when an epidemiologist doth 

protest too much. Protein is chemically complex; or more precisely, 

there are hundreds of different proteins that might have opposite 

effects in the body. There are twenty amino acids that make up these 

proteins and they, in turn, have many individual effects and some work 



198 



Nutrition in Crisis 



together with the others. We are asked to believe that after they are 

digested, our bodies can tell which amino acids came from animals and 

which from vegetables. 


It is not even clear that most of us eat a lot of meat. Protein tends to be a 

stable part of the diet. There was no change in protein consumption in the 

last forty years during the period of the obesity and diabetes epidemic. We 

might eat more than recommended by health agencies, but their creden¬ 

tials are up for renewal. In fact many individuals, particularly the elderly, 

don’t get enough meat. Given the complexities, it seems that the burden of 

proof should be on those who want to show the dangers of meat. Both the 

scientific and popular press, however, give you the idea that meat should be 

considered guilty until proven innocent. 


Red meat, in particular, is “linked to” just about every disease imag¬ 

inable, including diabetes, where if anything, it is likely to be beneficial. 

The lipophobes’ approach is epidemiological and their numerous papers 

have a common theme: find a weak association and claim that if the risk, 

no matter how miniscule, is multiplied by the whole population, we can 

save thousands of lives by reducing meat consumption. Here, again, I am 

asking you to accept the idea that the best and the brightest are not doing 

acceptable science. Using the principles from the previous chapters, I will 

analyze a couple of specific papers. These reports provide examples of the 

particular errors that you can look for when trying to decide if a scientific 

paper is valid or not. 


One important idea: All of statistics is subjective. Assumptions are 

made and numbers are calculated from those assumptions. The numbers 

are meaningful only if the assumptions fit the question to be answered. 

You have to remember that statistical significance is a mathematical 

term, meaning that differences between an experimental group and the 

controls in a particular experiment did not arise by chance. It does not 

mean, however, that those differences are of sufficient magnitude or are of 

sufficient reliability that if the experiment were repeated it would not turn 

out differently. It does not mean that the differences have any practical or 

clinical significance, either. Til begin with an older red meat study, which 

illustrates the limitations of relative risk and odds ratios. This particular 

case has a punch line, and Til show you that the whole paper was probably 

some kind of flimflam. 



Red Meat and the New Puritans 



199 



Red Meat Scare of 2009 


“Daily Red Meat Raises Chances of Dying Early” was the headline in the 

March 24,2009, issue of the Washington Post? This scare story was accom¬ 

panied by a photo of a gloved hand slicing beef with a scalpel-like knife, 

probably intended to evoke a CSI autopsy (although it still looked pretty 

good to me, if slightly overcooked). I don’t know the reporter, Rob Stein, 

but I don’t think we’re talking about anyone on the level of Woodward 

and Bernstein. For those too young to remember Watergate, the reporters 

from the Post were encouraged to “follow the money” by Deep Throat, 

their anonymous whistleblower. The movie Fat Head suggests the variation 

—more appropriate here—that we “follow the data.” 


In the strange world of nutrition, scandalous behavior is right out in 

the open. Researchers don’t like to be whistleblowers, because unless the 

issue has some major impact, a breakdown in research principles makes 

us all look bad. The missteps in published papers in nutrition, however, 

require no insider information to pick apart. The Washington Post story was 

based on a study, “Meat Intake and Mortality,” published in the profes¬ 

sional medical journal Archives of Internal Medicine by Rashmi Sinha and 

coauthors. 3 It got a certain amount of press at the time of publication, but 

following the usual pattern, it soon disappeared from view into the pile 

of “accumulating evidence” that meat would kill you. Nobody looked at it 

closely. Certainly not Rob Stein. To be fair, it’s not his fault. It takes time 

to analyze such papers and he was likely assigned to a flower show the 

next day. Although it is not unreasonable that he took the authors at their 

word, it is worth looking at the details now. When analyzing studies on the 

risk of a diet or drugs, looking back to Bradford Hill’s criteria, we should 

expect that there first be specificity. (Recall how Hill emphasized that it 

was lung and nose cancer specifically that were associated with cigarette 

smoking.) However, specificity is exactly what was lacking in Sinha et al.’s 

paper, whose main outcome measure was all-cause mortality—that is, the 

number of deaths from any disease. If a paper is about all-cause mortality, 

then the check on specificity is lost and we should be sure that this is for 

real. Unsurprisingly, the conclusion will be that the paper fails to make a 

good case for the danger of red meat, and in fact, its greatest virtue may be 

in showing you how to see through the flaws that populate the literature. 



200 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Common Sense and Experience First 


Your best bet in dealing with potential bias is to demand that, no matter 

how complicated the statistics are, the results should not violate common 

sense—or if they do seem 

to be counterintuitive, 

that they are convincingly 

justified. The further the 

conclusion deviates from 

common sense or previ¬ 

ous established results, 

the greater the demand 

for that justification. The 

salient fact about red meat 

is that during the thirty years that we describe as the obesity and diabetes 

epidemic, overall protein intake was relatively constant, and red meat 

consumption actually went down by a significant degree. As previously 

shown in chapter 1, almost all of the caloric increase in that period was 

due to an increase in carbohydrates. Fat, if anything, went down. However, 

during this period, consumption of almost everything else went up: Wheat 

and corn, of course, skyrocketed, but so did fruits and vegetables. The two 

notable foods whose consumption went down were red meat and eggs. In 

addition, we need to remember that much research shows the benefits of 

replacing carbohydrate with protein, especially for the elderly. 4 So, here’s 

something that you can use as a rule in analyzing papers in the literature. 


Simple Statistics: Who's Likely to Die? 


The paper by Rashmi Sinha et al. 5 is an observational study of the type 

discussed in the previous chapter: They tried to match outcomes for differ¬ 

ent groups in an existing population with their dietary behavior. In terms 

of informing the public, the report in the Washington Post is quite a bit 

more accessible than the original paper. It reads: 


Researchers analyzed data from 545,653 predominantly white 


volunteers, ages 50 to 71, participating in the National Institutes 



Principle 1 


Biology, common sense, and 

experience come before statis¬ 

tics. Do the results make sense? 



Red Meat and the New Puritans 



201 



of Health-AARP Diet and Health Study. In 1995, the subjects 

filled out detailed questionnaires about their diets, including 

meat consumption. Over the next 10 years, 47,976 men and 

23,276 women died. 


So the risk, or probability, of dying if you were in the study population 

is easy to calculate from the Washington Post report: overall probability 

= (number of people who 

died) / (all the people in 

the study). That is equal to 


(47,976 + 23,276)/545,653 Principle 2 


= 0.13 or 13 percent. This Are the results meaningful— 


is a good reference point, that is, large numbers? 


something that we re sure of 

before anybody starts doing 

the statistics. The risk is not 


great. Only slightly more than one in ten people died in the course of the 

experiment. The article goes on to say: 


Those who ate the most red meat—about a quarter-pound a 

day—were more likely to die of any reason, and from heart 

disease and cancer in particular, than those who ate the least— 

the equivalent of a couple of slices of ham a day. Among women, 

those who ate the most red meat were 36 percent more likely to 

die for any reason, 20 percent more likely to die of cancer and 

50 percent more likely to die of heart disease. Men who ate the 

most meat were 31 percent more likely to die for any reason, 22 

percent more likely to die of cancer and 27 percent more likely 

to die of heart disease. 


That sounds fairly scary, but is it? By this point you might be experienc¬ 

ing an emotional reaction to relative risk. This is almost always misleading. 

So, let’s do what scientists call a back-of-the-envelope calculation to see if 

we can get meaningful numbers. You can skip this if you don’t like math, 

but its really not bad—just high school algebra. We have the overall risk of 

mortality: 13 percent. Now, for both men and women, the increase in risk 



202 



Nutrition in Crisis 



from eating meat is about 33 percent (36 percent for women; 31 percent 

for men), so if N people died in the low-meat group, then 1.33iV died 

among the high-meat group. Together they make up 13 percent of the 

total, so we have: (N+ 1.33iV) = 2.33N= 13 percent. If you solve for A you 

get 0.0558, or about 5.6 percent. From there we can determine that the risk 

in the low-meat group is 5.6 percent, and the risk in the high-meat group 

is 7.4 percent. So the absolute difference in risk is only 1.8 percent. Before 

you even look at the original paper, we are now talking about a difference 

in risk in the ballpark of a few percent. Your steak is already sounding a 

lot better. 


What we are trying to do here is to go beyond relative risk and get at 

absolute risk. The dire warning that “those who ate the most red meat 

were 36 percent more likely to die for any reason” lost its kick when 

we did an absolute risk calculation. Why? Obviously, when it is a case 

of relative anything, you need to know: Relative to what? In diseases 

with low prevalence, or events with low probability, relative risk is almost 

always misleading. 



What Was Measured? 


Another problem is that the paper says “A 124-item food frequency 

questionnaire . . . was completed at baseline.” Many are suspicious of 

food questionnaires because people might not accurately report what 

they ate. While this can be a source of error, the amount of error 

depends on how the data is interpreted. All scientific measurements 

have error. Here the problem is the word “baseline.” The study was 

started in 1995, continued for ten years, and mortality was followed 

during this period. This means that some people died as long as ten 

years after their reported food intake. The take-home question here: 

Are you eating the same thing you were eating ten years ago? More to 

the point, with all the kvetching about red meat in the media, is there 

a chance somebody in this study reduced red meat consumption? Is 

there a chance that they did it halfway through the study and would 

have appeared in a different group if data had been collected for them 

at that halfway point? To simplify even further: If I get sick, it is due to 

the junk I ate in college? 



Red Meat and the New Puritans 



203 



But, let's take them at their word and assume the study is okay. What do 

we learn from the outcome? The damned statistics, as Mark Twain might 

have called them, go like this: The study population of 322,263 men and 

223,390 women was broken up into five groups (quintiles) according to 

meat consumption, with the highest group taking in about seven times 

as much as the lowest group. The groups were compared according to a 

statistic called hazard ratio (HR), which, as explained below, in cases like 

this is more or less the same as relative risk. The authors report that the 

HR for eating red meat every day compared to eating red meat rarely is 

1.44.This is pretty weak. Before discussing further, Til explain the different 

terms for expressing risk. Again, the math is simple, but you can skip ahead 

if you're daunted. 


Understanding OR, RR, and HR 


The common measures of relative outcomes in comparing a nutritional 

or medical intervention with controls are odds ratio (OR), relative risk 

(RR), and hazard ratio (HR). The good news is that for most of the stud¬ 

ies relevant to what were 

doing here, these statisti¬ 

cal measures are all pretty 

much the same (because 

the incidence of disease 

or other outcome is low). 


As described above, “risk” 

in medicine is the prob¬ 

ability of a given outcome, 

and relative risk is what it 

sounds like—the probability 

of an outcome occurring in 

one group compared to the probability of it occurring in a second group. 


Let’s break things down. Probability is equal to the number of particular 

outcomes divided by the total number of possible outcomes. We can look 

at some examples from the world of gambling, using the word winning as 

a stand-in for any particular outcome, such as getting sick or losing a target 

amount of weight: 



Principle 3 


If the effect is not large, then 

the data have to be very reliable. 

Conversely, if the data have big 

potential error, then the conclu¬ 

sion must be substantial. 



204 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Probability = Number of ways of winning/Total possible outcomes 


Probability of drawing = 4 aces/52 total cards = 1/13 = 0 .0769 

an ace from a fair deck 


Probability of drawing the _ j ^ of ^53 tMll caris , 1/s2 ,0.0192 

ace of spades from a fair deck 


In some cases, rather than the probability, the odds might be reported. 

Odds are slightly different from probability: 



Odds = Number of ways of winning/Number of ways of not winning 

Odds of drawing an, 4 aces/48 non _ aces cards , 1/12 . 0 . 0833 

ace from a fair deck 



Odds of drawing the ace 

of spades from a fair deck 



= 1 ace of spades / 



51 n ° n_aceof =1 /5 1= 0.0196 

spades cards 



As the likelihood of an event occurring diminishes, the odds and the 

probability move closer together and are used similarly in conversation. 

This is evident in the examples above: The probability of drawing the ace of 

spades from a fair deck is 1 out of 52, or .0192, while the odds are 1 out of 

51, or .0196. Because the event is unlikely to occur, the difference between 

odds and probability becomes almost negligible. 


In an experiment comparing outcomes between two groups, for example 

cancer in meat eaters and cancer in vegetarians, you might report relative 

risk (RR): 



(Probability of an (Probability of 


Relative Risk = . . x 


outcome m group 1 ; outcome in group 2) 


Relative risk of drawing any ace, compared to drawing an ace of spades: 

(4/54) / (1/54) = 4 


In a medical experiment you might not be able to wait until the 

experiment is over to calculate the probability of a given outcome. This 

is where hazard comes into play. Hazard is the same as the probability, 

but measured over fixed time intervals. For example, you might measure 



Red Meat and the New Puritans 



205 



how many people get sick in the first month in each group in a study. 

Hazard is a rate and is the probability of “winning” for a fixed time 

period. There is slightly more to hazard, mathematically, but in most 

papers it is reported as the hazard ratio (HR) between two interven¬ 

tions. The bottom line in these cases is that you can think of HR as the 

same as RR. 


It is important to realize that once you calculate RR, HR, or OR, you 

have lost track of how much absolute risk there was to begin with. If a 

paper tells you the RR of a disease, you don’t know whether it is a rare 

disease or one that everybody has. Bottom line: In reading a scientific 

paper, you can take RR, HR, and OR as roughly the same. They all 

provide a comparison between how likely two events are to occur. The 

big caveat is that you have to make sure you know what the individual 

probability of each event is. The problem is described well in the follow¬ 

ing narrative. 


You are in Las Vegas. There are two blackjack tables, and for some reason 

they have different probabilities of paying out (different number of decks, 

for example). The probability of winning at Table One is 1 in 100 hands, 

or 1 percent. At Table Two the probability of winning is 1 in 80 hands, or 

about 1.27 percent. The ratio of the probabilities (RR of winning) is 1.27 

/1,0 = 1.27. (A ratio of 1 would mean that there is no difference between 

the tables.) Suppose all that you know is the RR of 1.27. Right off, some¬ 

thing is wrong: You are missing a lot of information. One gambling table 

is definitely better than the other, but you have no way of knowing whether 

the odds at either table are particularly good in the first place. 


Suppose, however, that you did get a glimpse at the real info and you 

could find out what the absolute odds are at the different tables: 1 percent at 

Table One and 1.27 percent at Table Two. Does that help? Well, it depends 

on who you are. For the guy who is sitting at the black-jack table when 

you go up to sleep in your room at the hotel, and who is still there when 

you come down for the breakfast buffet, he is going to be much better off 

at Table Two. He will play hundreds of hands and the slightly better odds 

ratio of 1.27 is likely to pay off. On the other hand, imagine that you are 

somebody who will take the advice of my cousin, the statistician, who says 

to just go and play one hand for the fun of it, just to see if the universe loves 

you (that’s what gamblers are really trying to find out). You’re going to 



206 



Nutrition in Crisis 



play the hand, win or lose, and then go off and do something else. Does it 

matter which table you play at? Obviously it doesn’t. The odds ratio doesn’t 

tell you anything useful because your chances of winning are pretty slim 

either way. 



Differences in Absolute Risk 


Returning to the original red meat paper, there are a number of differ¬ 

ent “models” (reworking of the data) and there are mind-numbing tables 

giving you the different hazard ratios. Using the worst case HR between 

high and low red meat intakes for men, we get HR = 1.48, or, as they like 

to report in the media, 48 percent higher risk of dying from all causes. It 

sounds bad, but wait: What is the absolute difference in risk? Well, the 

paper says that the whole group of 322,263 men was divided into five 

subgroups (quintiles) of 64,453 people each. (Note that you usually have 

to dig this information out of tables. Authors don’t always make it easy to 

find the data, which is itself cause for suspicion.) The people who don’t eat 

much red meat had 6,437 deaths, or 10.0 percent by population. The big 

meat eaters must then have had 14.8 percent deaths. That’s an absolute 

difference of only 5 percent. The authors then corrected for some things 

that might have contributed to the outcome, bringing the absolute differ¬ 

ence for men down to a mere 3 percent. 


Bottom line: There is an absolute difference in risk of about 3 percent. 

It is unreasonable to think that this represents a call to change your life. 

Remember, too, that this is for big changes—like six or seven times as 

much meat. So, what is a meaningful HR? For comparison, the HR for 

smoking versus not smoking, with regard to lung disease, was about 20. 

For heavy smokers, the HR was about 30. Going back to Bradford Hill’s 

criteria: “First upon my list I would put the strength of the association.” 

Relative risk does not have meaning by itself. You must know the changes 

in absolute risk. Another common way of looking at the data that is more 

meaningful is the number needed to treat (NNT), which is the reciprocal 

of the absolute risk. For our 3 percent risk in Sinha’s red meat study, you 

would have to treat twenty to thirty people to save one life. That’s not great 

but it’s something. Or is it? 



Red Meat and the New Puritans 



207 



What About Public Health? Good News or Not? 


Many people would say that, sure, for a single person, red meat might not 

make a difference, but if the population reduced meat by half, we could save 

thousands of lives. At this point, before you and your family take part in a big 

experiment to save health statistics in the country, you have to apply Principles 

2 and 3. You have to ask how strong the relations are, and to understand how 

good the data is, you must look for things that would not be expected to have 

a correlation. “There was an increased risk associated with death from injuries 

and sudden death with higher consumption of red meat in men but not in 

women,” which sounds like we are dealing with a good deal of randomness. 


More important, what is the risk in reducing meat intake? The data 

don’t really tell you that. Unlike cigarettes, where there is little reason to 

believe that anybody’s lungs benefit from cigarette smoke, we know that 

there are many benefits to protein, especially if it replaces carbohydrate in 

the diet, especially for the elderly, and especially for all kinds of people. So 

with odds ratios around 1, you are almost as likely to benefit from adding 

red meat as you are reducing it. Technically, it is called a two-tailed distri¬ 

bution, which is to say that things can change in both directions. The odds 

still favor things getting worse but it is a risk in both directions. You are at 

the gaming tables. You don’t get your chips back. If you bet on reducing red 

meat and it does not reduce your risk, it will increase your risk. 



The Fine Print: The Smoking Gun 


In writing this chapter, which was based on a blog post, I went back to the 

original paper to check the calculations. I had used the numbers for men 

because it was a worst case (and it still has the problems that I described), 

but in recalculating things, I looked at the numbers for women as well.The 

data unfolded like this. 


The population was again broken up into five groups, or quintiles. The 

lower numbered quintiles are for the lowest consumption of red meat. 

Looking at the reported data on all-cause mortality, there were 5,314 deaths 

for low consumption. When you go up to quintile 5—that is, highest red meat 

consumption—there are 3,752 deaths. What? The more red meat, the lower 

the death rate? In other words, the raw data show the opposite of the conclusion 



208 



Nutrition in Crisis 



of the paper. There were fewer deaths at high red meat. The confounders are 

listed in the legend to the figure. For the “basic model,” the data were corrected 

for race and total energy intake, and risk went up. Why? We can’t tell if we 

cant see what the effect was. Do you have the sense of flimflam? 


A useful way to look at this data is from the standpoint of conditional 

probability. We ask: What is the probability of dying in this experiment 

if you consume a high-meat diet? The answer is simply the number of 

people who both died during the experiment and were big meat-eaters 

(Q5) divided by the total number of individuals in Q5. Here’s the calcula¬ 

tion: 3,752/(223,3905) = 0.0839, or about 8 percent. If you are not a big 

meat-eater, your risk is (5,314 + 5,081+ 4,734 + 4,395)/(0.8 x 223,390) = 

0.109, or about 11 percent. 


This paper tested the hypothesis that red meat is associated with all¬ 

cause mortality. The data showed that it wasn’t. Meat wasn’t a risk unless 

you dragged in other factors: education, marital status, family history of 

cancer, body mass index, smoking history using smoking status (never, 

former, current), time since quitting for former smokers, physical activity, 

alcohol intake, vitamin supplement user, fruit consumption, and vegetable 

consumption. All of these have to be added in to make the authors’ conclu¬ 

sion true. Once you do that, though, you have to ask why you would single 

out red meat among these other ten inputs as the key variable. Wouldn’t it 

be better to find out which of these factors had the biggest effect? What I 

offer here is a professional scientist’s view, and I try to make my description 

dispassionate and not insulting, but what is this if not deception? 



What Is It About Red Meat? 


Red meat isn’t a chemical, so what is it about the red meat that upsets people? 

The meat? The red? To be fair to the authors, they also studied white meat, 

which they found by the same prestidigitation mostly beneficial. But what 

about potatoes? Cupcakes? Breakfast cereal? Are these completely neutral? 

If we ran all these factors through the same computer, what would we see? 

Unspoken in everybody’s mind is saturated fat, that Rasputin of nutritional 

risk factors who will come after you despite enough bullets in its body to have 

killed several scientific theories. Maybe it wasn’t the red meat per se but the 

way it was procured. Maybe it’s ritual slaughter that conferred eternal life (or 



Red Meat and the New Puritans 



209 



lack of it) on the consumer. For a gripping description of the total spiritual 

effect of bad behavior, nothing is as harrowing as the Isaac Bashevis Singer 

stories equating meat eating with other sins of the flesh. Finally, there is the 

elephant in the room: carbohydrate. Basic biochemistry suggests that a roast 

beef sandwich might have a different effect than roast beef in a lettuce wrap. 


The Right Questions 


My hope is that you will come away from this chapter with some basic 

rules for dealing with the scientific literature—questions to ask and prin¬ 

ciples to apply: 


1. Do the results make sense? Biology comes before statistics. Experience 

comes before statistics. 


2. Is there a big association? Are we talking about meaningful, that is, large 

numbers? 


3 . What was measured? If the effect is not large, then the data have to be 

very reliable. If the data have big potential error, then the conclusion 

must be substantial. 


In nutrition, as in other fields, recommendations are often tinged by 

the personal preferences of those dishing them out. In combination with 

the dogmatic state of government and private recommendations, it takes 

some work to know if you are reading a meaningful study. The point of this 

chapter, and of this entire section of the book, is that you have the right to 

ask for, and the author has the obligation to provide, clear explanations. The 

practical application of Hills criteria is that results should make sense in 

terms of magnitude and what you know about biology. You should also be 

very suspicious if only relative risk is reported, even if just in the Abstract. 

Remember, Alice has 30 percent more money in the bank than Bob, but 

we don’t know whether she is rich. 


You can usually calculate absolute risk (the number of cases divided by 

the number of participants) so you can see if the report is about something 

that is very rare. The specific case in this chapter, the effect of red meat, fails 

to meet the criterion of meaningful associations and seems to be deployed 

to distract from the real culprit, the real elephant in the room: carbohydrate. 



210 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Harvard: 


Making Americans Afraid of Red Meat 


“There was a sense of Jeji-vu about the paper by Pan, et al. 

entitled 'Red meat consumption and mortality; results from 2 

prospective cohort studies’ 6 that came out in April of 2012.” 

That s what I wrote online in May of 2012. Other bloggers 

worked it over pretty well, so I ignored the paper for the most 

part. Then I came across a remarkable article from the Harvard 

Health Blog. Titled “Study Urges Moderation in Red Meat 

Intake,” it was about the Pan et al. study and it described how 

the “study linking red meat and mortality lit up the media.” It 

claimed that “headline writers had a field day, with entries like 

‘Red meat death study/ ‘Will red meat kill you?’ and ‘Singing 

the blues about red meat/” This was too much for me. 


What was odd about the post from the Harvard blog was 

that “the field day for headline writers” was all described from 

a distance, as if the study by Pan et al. (and the content of the 

Harvard blogpost itself) hadn’t come from Harvard, but was 

rather a natural phenomenon, similar to the way every seminar 

on obesity begins with a graphic of the state-by-state progres¬ 

sion of obesity as if it were some kind of meteorological event. 


The reference to “headline writers,”1 think, was intended to 

conjure images of sleazy tabloid publishers like the ones who 

are always pushing the limits of First Amendment rights in 

the old Law & Order episodes. The Harvard blogpost itself, 

however, was not any less exaggerated. It is not true that the 

Harvard study urged moderation. In fact, the article admitted 

that the original paper “sounded ominous. Every extra daily 

serving of unprocessed red meat (steak, hamburger, pork, etc.) 

increased the risk of dying prematurely by 13%. Processed red 

meat (hot dogs, sausage, bacon, and the like) upped the risk 

by 20%. ” 



Red Meat and the New Puritans 



211 



That is what the paper urged. Not moderation. Prohibition. 

“Increased the risk of dying prematurely by 13%. ” Who wants 

to buck odds like that? Who wants to die prematurely? 


It wasn’t just the media. Critics in the blogosphere were also 

working overtime deconstructing the study. Among the faults 

cited was the reporting of relative risk. Once again: Relative 

risk is relative. It doesn’t tell you what the risk is to begin 

with. Relative risk destroys information. The extreme example 

remains that you can double your odds of winning the lottery 

if you buy two tickets instead of one. So why do people keep 

reporting it? One reason, of course, is that it makes your work 

look more significant than it is, but if you don’t report the abso¬ 

lute change in risk, you might be scaring people about risks that 

aren’t real. The nutritional establishment is not good at facing 

their critics, but in this case, Harvard admitted that they didn’t 

wish to push back against their detractors. 


Nolo Contendere 


Having turned the media loose to scare the American public, 

Harvard admitted that the bloggers were correct. The Harvard 

Health Blog allocated to having reported “relative risks, 

comparing death rates in the group eating the least meat with 

those eating the most.” “The absolute risks,” they admitted, 

“sometimes help tell the story a bit more clearly. These numbers 

are somewhat less scary” Why not try to tell the story as clearly 

as possible in the original article then? 


Anyway, there was a table on the Harvard Health Blog 

that presented the raw data. This is what you need to know. 

Unfortunately, the Harvard Health Blog didn’t actually calcu¬ 

late the absolute risk for you. You’d think that they would want 

to make up for Dr. Pan scaring you. After all, an allocution is 

supposed to remove doubt about the details of the crimes. Let’s 

calculate the absolute risk. It s not hard. 



212 



Nutrition in Crisis 



As previously, risk is a probability: that is, number of cases 

divided by total number of participants. Looking at the data for 

the men first, the risk of death with three servings per week is 

equal to 12.3 cases per 1,000 people, or 1.23 percent. Now going 

to fourteen servings a week the risk of death is 13 cases per 1,000, 

or 1.3 percent. So, for men, the absolute difference in risk is less 

than 0.1 percent. Definitely less scary. In fact, not scary at all. 


Still, at least according to the public health professionals, 

the risk could add up for millions of people. Or could it? We 

have to step back and ask what is predictable about showing a 

change of less than one-tenth of 1 percent risk. It means that a 

small unpredictable event—a couple of guys getting hit by cars 

in one or another of the groups—might throw the whole thing 

off (remember we’re talking total deaths). Many other things 

have a lower risk than that, but might not have been considered. 

Maybe a handful of guys in upscale, vegetarian social circles lied 

about their late night trips to Dinosaur Barbecue. The number 

has no real-world significance. When can you scale up a small 

outcome? If the outcome number is small, and you want to scale 

it up, it must be secure and have little room for error. 


There is an underlying theme here.There is the possibility that the 

mass of epidemiology studies from the Harvard School of Public 

Health and other groups are simply not real. Poor understanding 

of science, cognitive dissonance, or something else altogether might 

be the cause. Whatever it is, the progression of epidemiologic stud¬ 

ies showing that meat causes diabetes, that sugar causes gout, all 

with low-hazard ratios—that is, small association size—might he 

meaningless. It’s discouraging and almost impossible to believe. A 

big piece of medical research is a house of cards. 


Who Paid for This and What Should Be Done 


We paid for it. Pan et al were funded in part by six National 

Institutes of Health grants and NIH is still paying for it. The 



Red Meat and the New Puritans 



213 



latest production from this group now emphasizes the “change 

in meat consumption. 07 Remarkably, my objections to this 

paper were published as a letter to the editor. 8 My main point 

was that “Red meat consumption decreased as T2DM [type 2 

diabetes mellitus] increased during the past 30 years.” 


It is hard to believe with all the flaws pointed out by myself 

and others, and in the end, admitted by the Harvard Health 

Blog, that this work was subject to any meaningful peer review. 

A plea of no contest does not imply negligence or intent to 

do harm, but something is wrong. There is the clear attempt 

to influence the dietary habits of the population. This cannot 

sensibly be justified by an absolute risk reduction of less than 

one-tenth of 1 percent, especially given that others have made 

the case that some part of the population, particularly the 

elderly, might not get adequate protein. The need for an over¬ 

sight committee of impartial scientists is the most reasonable 

conclusion from Pan et al. 






-CHAPTER 15- 



The Seventh Egg 


When Studies Defy Common Sense 



S tepping back and looking at the recent nutritional literature, I am 

struck by the miracle of life. How could humans have evolved in 

the face of threats from red meat, eggs, and even shaving? (The 

Caerphilly Prospective Study shows you the dangers of shaving ... or is it 

the dangers of not shaving? 1 ) With 28 percent greater risk of diabetes here 

and 57 percent greater risk of heart disease there, how could our ances¬ 

tors ever have come of childbearing age? Considering the daily revelations 

from the Harvard School of Public Health, showing the Scylla of saturated 

fat and the Charybdis of sugar between which our forefathers sailed, it is 

a miracle that we're here. 


These sensational stories that the popular media writes about, are 

they real? They are, after all, based on scientific papers. Although not all 

members of the media have the expertise to decipher them, reporters 

generally talk to the researchers—and the papers must have gone through 

peer review in the first place, right? However, as the previous chapters 

suggest, the gatekeepers, as we think of peer reviewers, are less vigilant 

than they should be. In fact, many papers that are published in the major 

medical journals defy common sense. While it is hard to believe, the medi¬ 

cal literature really does have a remarkably high degree of error. Medical 

publications are burdened with many examples of misinterpretation and 

poor understanding of scientific design and analysis. While there is little 

outright falsification of data, the researchers are not always doing credible 

science. How can the consumer decide? 


I am going to illustrate the problem with the example of a paper by L. 

Djousse: “Egg Consumption and Risk of Type 2 Diabetes in Men and 

Women.” 2 In the study by Djousse et al., participants were asked how 



216 



Nutrition in Crisis 



many eggs they ate and then, ten years later, it was determined whether 

they had developed diabetes. If they had, it was assumed to be because 

of the number of eggs. Is this for real? Do eggs really cause changes in 

your body that accumulate until you develop a disease—a disease that is, 

after all, primarily one of carbohydrate intolerance? Type 2 diabetes, recall, 

is due to impaired response of the body to the insulin produced by beta 

cells of the pancreas, as well as a progressive deterioration of the insulin- 

producing cells. Common sense tells us its unlikely that eggs could play a 

major role, but it is still important to understand the methodology and see 

if there is a something that justifies this obvious departure from common 

sense. Again, Hills principles might be useful. 


What did the experimenters actually do? First, subjects were specifically 

asked “to report how often, on average, they had eaten one egg during the 

past year,” and subjects where thus classified into one the following catego¬ 

ries of egg consumption: 0, <1 per week, 1 per week, 2-4 per week, 5-6 per 

week, and 7+ eggs per week. The researchers collected these data every other 

year for ten years. With these data in hand, they then followed subjects 

“from baseline until the first occurrence of one of the following: (a) diag¬ 

nosis of type 2 diabetes; (b) death; or (c) censoring date, the date of receipt 

of the last follow-up questionnaire,” which for men was up to twenty years. 


Take a second and consider the last year of your life. Is it possible that 

you might not be able to remember whether you had one versus two eggs 

per week on average during the year? Is there any possibility that some of 

the subjects who were diagnosed with diabetes ten years after their report 

on egg consumption had changed their eating pattern over the course of 

that decade-long experiment? Are you eating the same food you ate ten 

years ago? Quick, how many eggs per week did you eat last year? 



The Golden Rule Again 


Right off, there is a problem in people accurately reporting what they ate. 

This is a limitation of many—probably most—nutritional studies, and 

while it can be a source of error, it is really a question of how you interpret 

the data. All scientific measurements have error. You simply have to be sure 

that the results that you are trying to find do not depend on any greater 

accuracy than the data that you have collected. Eyeballing the paper by 



The Seventh Egg 



217 



Djousse et al., we see that there are no figures, which is already a suspi¬ 

cious sign. We would have expected, at least, a graph of the number of 

eggs consumed versus the number of cases of diabetes. The results, instead, 

are stated in the abstract of the paper in the form of this mind-numbing 

conclusion (don't actually try to read this; it is merely an illustration of the 

tedious writing in the medical literature): 


Compared with no egg consumption, multivariable adjusted 

hazard ratios (95% Cl) for type 2 diabetes were 1.09 (0.87-1.37), 


1.09 (0.88-1.34), 1.18 (0.95-1.45), 1.46 (1.14-1.86), and 1.58 

(1.25-2.01) for consumption of <1, 1, 2-4, 5-6, and 7+ eggs^ 

week, respectively, in men (p for trend <0.0001). Corresponding 

multivariable hazard ratios (95% Cl) for women were 1.06 

(0.92-1.22), 0.97 (0.83-1.12), 1.19 (1.03-1.38), 1.18 (0.88- 

1.58), and 1.77 (1.28-2.43), respectively (p for trend <0.0001). 


What does all this mean? Very little. These “statistical shenanigans” are, 

in fact, an argument against a correlation. If you look at the paragraph, 

almost every number that you see is very close to 1. Without going through 

a detailed analysis, you can simply extract from the tables some simple infor¬ 

mation: There were 1,921 men who developed diabetes. Of these, 197 were 

in the high egg consumption group, or about 10 percent. For women, there 

were 2,112 cases of diabetes, of whom 46 were high egg consumers, or a little 

more than 2 percent. To me this suggests that diabetes is associated with 

something other than eggs, and that it is probably unjustified of the authors 

to conclude: “These data suggest that high levels of egg consumption (daily) 

are associated with an increased risk of type 2 diabetes in men and women.” 


What I described are the raw data, and as we saw in chapter 14, we have 

to consider confounders. In fact, if we analyzed the data in detail, we would 

find that the conclusion is actually poorly supported by the data, but lets 

take the authors' conclusion at face value for the sake of argument. 



The Seventh Egg 


If the authors' conclusion is correct, this means that there was no additional 

risk of diabetes from consuming 1 egg per week as compared to eating 



218 



Nutrition in Crisis 



none. Similarly, there was no additional risk in eating 2-4 eggs per week 

or 5-6 eggs per week. However if you upped your intake to 7 eggs or more 

per week, then thats it: You were at risk for diabetes. It makes me think of 

the movie The Seventh Seal , directed by Ingmar Bergman. Very popular in 

the fifties and sixties, these movies had a captivating if pretentious style: 

They sometimes seemed to be designed for the Woody Allen's parodies 

that would follow. One of the famous scenes in The Seventh Seal is the 

protagonist's chess game with Death. It requires only a little imagination 

to see the egg as the incarnation of the Grim Reaper. 



Mindless Statistics 


A study of 20,703 men and 36,295 women makes a very weak case for 

a connection between egg consumption and type 2 diabetes. Few of the 

people who developed diabetes were big egg eaters. The problem is the 

mindless use of statistics. If statistics ever go against common sense, it is 

the authors' responsibility to explain why—in detail and in the abstract. 


Sometimes, you can get a sense of how real the statistics are by looking 

for simple things: How many people were in the study and how many got 

sick? In other words, are we talking about a rare disease or one that had 

low probability? In the case of this particular experiment from Djousse et 

al., diabetes is a major health risk, but if you take 1,000 men for ten years, 

and only 1 in 10 might develop diabetes, you need to be sure there is a big 

difference between the group that followed the behavior you are looking at 

(egg consumption) and those who didn't. 



-CHAPTER 16- 



Intention- to-Treat 


What It Is and Why You Should Care 



T he medical literature has some strange customs, but nothing beats 

intention-to-treat (ITT), the most absurd and amusing statistical 

method that has appeared recently According to ITT, the data from 

a subject assigned at random to an experimental group must be included in 

the reported outcome data for that group, even if the subject does not follow 

the protocol, or even if they drop out of the experiment. In other words, it 

doesn't matter if you eat what the experimenter told you to eat—your lipid 

profile has to be included in the final report. It s easy enough to tell that the 

idea is counterintuitive if not completely idiotic—why would you include 

people who are not in the experiment in your data? The burden of proof in 

using such a method should rest with its proponents, but no such obligation 

is felt, and particularly in nutrition studies—such as comparisons of isoca¬ 

loric weight Toss diets—ITT is frequently used with no justification at all. 

Astoundingly, the practice is sometimes actually demanded by reviewers in 

the scientific journals. As one might expect, there is a good deal of controversy 

on this subject. Physiologists or chemists, or really anybody with scientific 

training, who hears about ITT requirements will usually walk away shaking 

their head or uttering some obvious reductio ad absurdum —for instance, “You 

mean, if nobody takes the pill, you report whether or not they got better 

anyway?”That’s exactly what it means. In this chapter Til describe a couple of 

interesting cases from the medical literature, and one relatively new instance 

Fosters two-year study of low-carbohydrate diets—to demonstrate the abuse 

of common sense that is the major characteristic of ITT. 


On the naive assumption that some people really didn't understand 

what was wrong with ITT—Ive been known to make a few elementary 

mistakes in my life—I wrote a paper on the subject. It received negative, 



220 



Nutrition in Crisis 



even hostile, reviews from two public health journals. I even got substan¬ 

tial grief from reviewers at Nutrition and Metabolism , where it was finally 

published. I was the editor at the time, and I had extensive communica¬ 

tions with one reviewer who was willing to forego anonymity. 


The title of my paper was “Intention-to-Treat. What Is the Question?” 

My point was that there’s nothing inherently wrong with ITT if you 

are explicit about what you are measuring. If you use ITT, you are really 

asking: What is the effect of assigning subjects to an experimental proto¬ 

col? However, is anybody really interested in what the patient was told to 

do rather than what they actually did? 


The practice of ITT comes from clinical trials, where you cant always 

tell whether patients have taken the recommended pills, just as in the real 

situation where you never know what people will do once they leave the 

doctor’s office. In that case you do an analysis based on your intention; that 

is, you have no other choice than to designate those who were assigned to the 

intervention as the “experimental group,” even though they might not have 

participated in the experiment. That’s what we always did without giving it a 

special name. When you do know who took the pill and who didn’t, however, 

there are two separate questions: Did they take the pill, and is the pill any 

good? That’s the data. You have to know both, and if you want to collapse 

them into one number, you have to be sure you make clear what you are talk¬ 

ing about. You lose information if you collapse efficacy and adherence into 

one number. It is common for the abstract of a paper to correctly state that 

the results are about subjects “assigned to a diet” but by the time the results 

are presented, the independent variable has usually become simply “the diet,” 

rather than “assignment to diet,” which most people would assume meant 

what people ate rather than what they were told to eat. Caveat lector. 


My paper on ITT was perhaps overkill. I made several different 

arguments, but a single commonsense argument gets to the heart of the 

problem. I’ll describe that first, along with a couple of real-world examples. 


Commonsense Argument Against 

Intention-to-Treat 


Consider an experimental comparison of two diets in which there is a 

simple, discrete outcome (e.g., a threshold amount of weight lost or 



Intention-to-Treat 



221 



Table 16.1. Hypothetical Results from the Thought Experiment for Analysis of Diets A and B 




Diet A 


Diet B 


Compliance (of 100 patients] 


50 


100 


Success (reached target) 


50 


50 


ITT success 


50/100 = 50% 


50/100 = 50% 


“Per protocol” (followed diet] success 


50/50 = 100% 


50/100 = 50% 



remission of an identifiable symptom.) Patients are randomly assigned 

to two different diets, diet A or diet B, and a target of, say, 5 kilograms 

of weight loss is considered success. As shown in table 16.1, half of the 

subjects in diet A are “compliers,” able to stay on the diet, while the other 

half are not. The half of the patients on diet A who were compliers were 

all able to lose the target 5 kilograms, while the noncompliers did not. In 

diet B, on the other hand, everybody stayed on the diet, but somehow only 

half were able to lose the required amount of weight. An ITT analysis 

shows no difference in the two outcomes—half of group A stayed on the 

diet and all lost weight, while in study B, everybody complied but only half 

had success. 


Now, suppose you are the doctor. With such data in hand, should you 

advise a patient: “Well, the diets are pretty much the same. It s largely up 

to you which you choose,” as required by ITT? Or, alternatively, looking at 

the raw data would the recommendation be: “Diet A is much more effec¬ 

tive than diet B, but people have trouble staying on it. If you can stay on 

diet A, it will be much better for you, so I would encourage you to see if you 

could find a way to do so.” You are the doctor. Which makes more sense? 


Diet A is obviously better, but it s hard for people to stay on it. This is 

one of the characteristics of ITT: It always makes the better diet look worse 

than it is. In the submitted manuscript, I made several arguments trying to 

explain that there are two factors. One of them, whether it actually works, 

is directly due to the diet. The other, whether you follow the diet, is under 

control of other factors (whether WebMD tells you that one diet or the 

other will kill you, whether the evening news makes you lose your appetite, 

etc.). I even dragged in a geometric argument because Newton had used 

one in the Principia : “A two-dimensional outcome space where the length 

of a vector tells how every subject did . . . ITT represents a projection 








222 



Nutrition in Crisis 



of the vector onto one axis, in other words collapses a two-dimensional 

vector to a one-dimensional vector, thereby losing part of the information.” 

Pretentious? Moi? 



Why You Should Care: Surgery or Medicine? 


Does your doctor actually read these academic studies using ITT? One 

can only hope not. Consider the analysis by David Newell of the Coronary 

Artery Bypass Surgery (CABS) trial. This paper is fascinating for the blan¬ 

ket, tendentious insistence, without any logical argument, on something 

that is obviously fundamentally foolish. Newell wrote: “The CABS research 

team was impeccable. They refused to do an as treated’ analysis: We have 

refrained from comparing all patients actually operated on with all not 

operated on: This does not provide a measure of the value of surgery.” 1 


You read it right. The results of surgery do not provide a measure of the 

value of surgery. 


In the CABS trial, patients were assigned to be treated with either 

medicine or surgery. The actual method used and the outcomes are shown 

in table 16.2. Looking at the table, you see the effects of assignments 

(ITT): 7.8 percent mortality for those assigned to receive medical treat¬ 

ment (29 deaths out of 373), and 5.3 percent mortality for those assigned 

to surgery (21 deaths of 371). On the other hand if you look at the outcomes 

of each treatment as actually used, it turns out that medical treatment had 

a mortality rate of 9.5 percent (33 deaths of 349), while among those who 


Table 16.2. Results from the CABS Trial from Newell 


Allocated medicine Allocated surgery 




Received 


Received 


Received 


Received 



surgery 


medicine 


surgery 


medicine 


Survived 2 years 


48 


296 


354 


20 


Died 



27 


15 



Total 


50 


323 


369 


26 



Source. David J Newell, "Intention-to-Treat Analysis: Implications for Quantitative 

and Qualitative Research,” International Journal of Epidemiology 23, no. 5 [1992] 

837-841 





Intention-to-Treat 



223 



actually underwent surgery, the mortality rate was only 4.1 percent (17 

deaths of 419). Mortality was less than half in surgery compared to medical 

treatment. Making such a simple statement, that surgery was better than 

medicine, Newell claimed, “would have wildly exaggerated the apparent 

value of surgery.”The “apparent value of surgery”? Common sense suggests 

that appearances are not deceiving. If you were one of the 16 people who 

were still alive because you chose surgery based on outcome (17 deaths) 

rather than because of ITT (33 deaths), you would think that it was the 

theoretical report of your death that had been exaggerated. 


The thing that is under the control of the patient and the physician, and 

that is not a feature of the particular modality, is getting the surgery actually 

done. Common sense dictates that a patient is interested in surgery, not the 

effect of being told that surgery is good. The patient has a right to expect 

that if they comply with the recommendation for surgery, the physician 

should try to avoid any mistakes from previous studies that prevented other 

patients from actually receiving the operation. In another defense of ITT, 

Hollis and Campbell made the somewhat cryptic statement: “Most types 

of deviations from protocol would continue to occur in routine practice.” 2 

This seems to be saying that the same number of people will always forget 

to take their medication and surgeons will continue to have exactly the 

same scheduling problems as in the CABS trial. ITT assumes that practi¬ 

cal considerations are the same everywhere and that any practitioner is 

equally capable, or incapable, as the original experimenter when it comes 

to getting the patient into the operating room. 


One might also ask what happens when two studies give different values 

from ITT analysis. In the extreme case, one might suggest that if the same 

operation were recommended at a hospital in Newcastle-upon-Tyne as 

opposed to a battlefield-in Iraq, the two ITT values would be different. So 

which is the appropriate one to attribute to that surgical procedure? 



The ITT Controversy 


Advocates of ITT see its principles as established and might dismiss a 

commonsense approach as naive. They usually say that removing noncom- 

pliers introduces bias by destroying the original randomization. In this, they 

are confusing the process of randomization with the criteria for inclusion. 



224 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Case Study: Vitamin E Supplementation 


A good case for the problems with ITT is a report on the value 

of antioxidant supplements. The abstract of the paper in ques¬ 

tion concluded that “there were no overall effects of ascorbic 

acid, vitamin E, or beta carotene on cardiovascular events among 

women at high risk for CVD.” This conclusion—that there was 

no effect of supplementation—was based on an ITT analysis, but 

on the fourth page of the paper one can see the remarkable effect 

of not counting subjects who didn’t comply: “Noncompliance 

led to a significant 13% reduction in the combined end point of 

CVD morbidity and mortality ... with a 22% reduction in MI 

. .,, a 27% reduction in stroke ... [, and] a 23% reduction in the 

combination of MI, stroke, or CVD death.” 


The media universally reported the conclusion from the 

abstract, namely, that there was no effect of vitamin E. This 

conclusion is correct if you think that you can measure the 

effect of vitamin E without taking the pill out of the bottle. 

(It s like the old joke about how you make a really dry martini: 

You don't remove the cap from the Vermouth bottle before 

pouring.) Does this mean that vitamin E is really of value? The 

data would certainly be accepted as valuable if the statistics 

were applied to a study of the value of, say, replacing barbecued 

pork with whole grain cereal. Certainly we can see that there is 

“no effect” when you tell a patient to take vitamin E, but that 

doesn’t answer the question that most people are interested in: 

What are the effects when the patient actually takes the vitamin f 



If you excluded people with diabetes from your study and only found out 

when the study started that one of the subjects actually had diabetes, like 

the tainted juries in courtroom dramas, you would be required to remove 

the individual anyway. You would not have included them if their diabetes 




Intention-to-Treat 



225 



came out in the voir dire, so you cant include them now. Of course, if 

you don’t know whether a subject has actually complied, you are forced to 

include everybody, and that is what we have always done when there is no 

choice. That is not the issue here.The question is: What happens when you 

know who did and who didn’t comply? 


The problem is not easily resolved. Statistics is not axiomatic: There is 

nothing analogous to the zeroth law (the idea of thermal equilibrium on 

which thermodynamics rests). All statistics rests on interpretation and 

intuition. If this is not appreciated—if you do not go back to consideration 

of exactly what the question is that you are asking—4t is easy to develop a 

dogmatic approach and insist on a particular methodology because it has 

become standard. 



Assumption versus Consumption 


Described in chapter 3 as the “shot heard round the world,” Gary Foster’s 

2002 dietary study found that after one year, a low-carbohydrate diet was 

substantially better than a low-fat diet for markers of cardiovascular disease. 

When it came to weight loss, however, while the low-carbohydrate diet 

was better at six months, the two diets were “the same at one year.” This, of 

course, was an effect of dwindling adherence in both the intervention and 

control groups, (although, in fact, the lipid markers were noticeably better 

on the low-carbohydrate group even at one year.) 


A follow-up to the landmark one-year study, “Weight and Metabolic 

Outcomes after 2 Years on a Low-Carbohydrate Versus Low-Fat Diet,” 3 

published in 2010, had a surprisingly limited impact. What went wrong? 

The first paper was revolutionary, and in the follow-up, the authors explicitly 

addressed the need for including a “comprehensive lifestyle modification 

program,” because the original was criticized for simply giving the people 

on the Atkins diet a copy of the popular book. The criticism was addressed, 

and the conclusion was the same. The low-carbohydrate group had better 

outcomes for most CVD risk factors, although again, there was no differ¬ 

ence in weight loss after two years. As stated in the conclusion: “Neither 

dietary fat nor carbohydrate intake influenced weight loss when combined 

with a comprehensive lifestyle intervention.”This is, after all, still the party 

line and should have been well-received by establishment nutrition. That 



226 



Nutrition in Crisis 



should have been a big win for the only-calories-count crowd. Why was 

there so little impact, and why is the paper so rarely cited? 


It is likely that the Zeitgeist has shifted since eight years ago. Strict 

scientific standards have suffered tremendous blows. The willingness to 

disregard the results of big expensive experiments and to ignore common 

sense is now much more widespread. Everybody knows—at this point— 

that in a big comparison trial, the low-carbohydrate diet will win in some 

way, and that it is likely that the authors will try to put a negative spin 

on things. As Elizabeth Nabel put it after the embarrassing Womens 

Health Initiative, failure: “Nothings changed.” Low-fat is still the name 

of the game, a phrase in the USDA guidelines, and a component of the 

Healthy Hunger-Free Kids Act school lunch program 4 endorsed by 

Michelle Obama. Press releases on such government programs quote a 

progression of “suits” with maudlin expressions of optimism about how 

well what I call “Fruits ’n Vegetables” are doing in improving obesity. There 

are numerous disclaimers about good fats and bad fats, so that you cant 

really hold them to anything. However, since Foster s first study, numerous 

low-carbohydrate trials have showed it to be superior to any competition. 

Foster s 2010 study was, thus, not the challenge that the original was. It 

should, however, have been given some attention, at least because it was 

a follow-up to the landmark first paper. In my opinion, it deserved more 

attention primarily because it represented a new low in misleading science. 

The conclusion of the paper: “Neither dietary fat nor carbohydrate intake 

influenced weight loss.” 


I admit that I had not read Fosters paper carefully before making 

the pronouncement that it was not very good. I was even upbraided by 

a student for such a rush to judgment. I explained that that is what I do 

for a living. I explained that I usually don’t have to spend a lot of time 

with a paper to get the general drift. I could easily see a couple of errors 

in methodology, which I describe below, but I was probably not totally 

convincing, so I went ahead and read the paper, which is quite a bit longer 

than usual. The main thing that I was looking for was information on the 

nutrients that were actually consumed, since it was their lack of effect that 

was the main point of the paper. The problem is that people feel that they 

can call anything that they want a low-carbohydrate diet, and of course, 

people really believe that being “assigned to a low-carbohydrate diet” is the 



Intention-to-Treat 



227 



same thing as consuming a low-carbohydrate diet. Frequently when you 

look carefully at these studies, it turns out the diets were similar, so it is not 

surprising that the results are the same. 


“Any Reasonably Intelligent High School Student” 


In a diet experiment, the food consumed should be right up front, but in this 

case I couldn’t find it at all. Foster s 2010 paper is quite long, with a tedious 

appendix on the lifestyle intervention, but I read the whole thing carefully. 

I really did. The data weren’t there. I was going to write to the authors when 

I found out—I think through somebody’s blog—that this paper had been 

covered in a story in the Los Angeles Times. As reported by Bob Kaplan: 


Of the 307 participants enrolled in the study, not one had their 

food intake recorded or analyzed by investigators. The authors 

did not monitor, chronicle or report any of the subjects’ diets. 


No meals were administered by the authors; no meals were 

eaten in front of investigators. There were no self-reports, no 

questionnaires. . . . The lead authors, Gary Foster and James 

Hill, explained in separate e-mails that self-reported data are 

unreliable and therefore they didn’t collect or analyze any. 


I confess to feeling a bit shocked. How can you say “neither dietary fat 

nor carbohydrate intake influenced weight loss” if you haven’t measured 

fat or carbohydrate? If you think that self-reported data are not good, 

then you cant make judgments about what was consumed. This would 

constitute a remarkable statement, since in fact the whole nutrition field 

runs on self-reported data. Are all the papers from the Harvard School of 

Public Health and all those epidemiology studies that rely on food records 

to be chucked out? 


What would have happened if the authors had actually measured the 

relevant data—if they had asked what people eat, which, as Kaplan put it, 

is “the single most important question . .. that any reasonably intelligent 

high school student would ask.” It’s not just bad experimental design. It is 

a question of what is on their mind. Do they not realize that it is totally 

inappropriate to say that fat or carbohydrate are not important if they 



228 



Nutrition in Crisis 



haven’t measured them? They might be so biased that they don’t see what 

is going on. In his first paper, Foster said in public, explicitly, that he set out 

to trash the Atkins diet. It’s not a good way to do science. You are supposed 

to try to trash your own theory and show that it survives. 



Beyond ITT: Fabricating Data 


Foster’s 2010 paper shows just how misleading ITT can be. Initially chas¬ 

tised for jumping to conclusions, I reread the paper carefully and one figure 

in particular caught my eye. The title: “Predicted Absolute Mean Change 

in Serum Triglycerides.” Predicted? That sounds strange. What about the 

real data? The figure indicates changes in triglycerides for the three-, six-, 

twelve-, and twenty-four-month time periods. 


Reduction in triglycerides is the hallmark of low-carbohydrate diets. 

Almost everybody on such a diet lowers their triglycerides (mine fell to half 

of the original value, a commonly reported outcome). In figure 16.IT, the 

difference in the levels of triglycerides between the two diets (shown by the 

double-headed arrow) was quite large at three or six months, the usual result. 

However, as the experiment continued, after twelve months, triglyceride 

values got closer and actually came together after twenty-four months. 


This seemed strange, so I realized I had to find out where the word 

predicted came from, and that meant reading the methods section and 

going over the statistical analysis on how the data had been handled. In 

general, as mentioned before, you usually only read the methods section 

in detail if you think that it is a problem, or if a new method has been 

introduced, or if you want to repeat the author’s experiment yourself. Large 

studies like this usually have a statistician, and they use standard methods 

whose details might or might not be understood by a nonstatistician (that’s 

me) and most of the authors. They are usually accepted at face value and it 

is assumed that authors have adequately explained to the statistician what 

the question is that they want to address. As I kept plowing through the 

statistical section, I found it increasingly tedious and difficult to read until 

I hit this passage: 


The previously mentioned longitudinal models preclude the 

use of less robust approaches, such as fixed imputation methods 



Triglyceride Difference Between Diets 03 Change in Triglyceride Level (mg/dL) 



Intention-to-Treat 



229 




-5 


-10 


-15 


-20 


-25 


-30 


-35 


-40 


-45 


-50 




12 


Month 



24 



25 



20 



15 



10 







Figure 16.1. A , The double-headed arrow represents the difference between the 

change in triglycerides on the two indicated arms of the study. Figure redrawn from 

Foster et al. B f The distance shown by the arrow on the y-axis in part A (difference 

between the low-carbohydrate and low-fat groups) is shown as a function of the 

number of subjects who dropped out of the study. 



















230 



Nutrition in Crisis 



(for example, last observation carried forward or the analysis of 

participants with complete data [that is, complete case analy¬ 

ses]). These alternative approaches assume that missing data are 

unrelated to previously observed outcomes or baseline covariates, 

including treatment (that is, missing completely at random). 


Data “missing completely at random”? Whats going on here? In a 

nutshell, this is another implementation of ITT. In the study, the authors 

used “data” from people who dropped out of the experiment. All they had 

to do was “assume that all participants who withdraw would follow first 

the maximum and then minimum patient trajectory of weight.” The key 

words are withdraw and assume . This is really a step beyond ITT, where 

you would include, for example, the weight of people who showed up to be 

weighed but had not actually followed one or another diet. Here, nobody 

showed up. There are no data. A pattern of behavior is assumed and data 

are—lets face it—made up. 


The world of nutrition puts big demands on irony and tongue-in-cheek, 

but the process in Foster's paper suggests that the results could, in theory, 

be fit to a model for a three-year study, or a ten-year study. As people 

dropped out you could “impute” the data. In some sense, you could do 

without any subjects at all. Nutrition experiments are expensive: Think of 

the money that could be saved if you didn’t have to put anybody on a diet 

and you could make up all the data. 


The Diet or the Lack of Compliance? 


It is odd that ITT is controversial. In fact, it’s odd that ITT exists at all. 

A reasonable way to deal with concern, however, that would satisfy every¬ 

body, is simply to publish both the ITT data and the data that include only 

the compilers, the so-called “per protocol” group—that is, the subjects that 

were actually in the experiment. This is what was done in the vitamin E 

study described in an earlier sidebar. It made the authors look bad, sure, but 

they did the right thing. Such data are missing from Foster’s paper. Given 

the high attrition rate, one could guess that the decline in performance in 

both groups was due to including “data” from the large number of people 

who failed to complete the study. It turns out we can test this. The number 



Intention-to-Treat 



231 



of people who dropped out is in the paper. To find out whether the decline 

in performance is due to including the made-up data from the dropouts, 

we can plot the difference in triglycerides between the two groups (the 

double-headed arrow in figure 16. L4, for each time point, against the 

number of people who discontinued treatment. 


Figure 16.15 gives you the answer. You can see a direct correlation 

between the number of dropouts and the group differences. The reason 

that “decreases in triglyceride levels were greater in the low-carbohydrate 

than in the low-fat group at 3 and 6 months but not at 12 or 24 months” 

was almost certainly because, with time, more and more people included 

in the data weren't on the low-carbohydrate diet, or any diet at all. ITT 

always makes the better diet look worse. 


This serves as an example of a case where correlation strongly implies 

causality: The expectation from previous literature is that there will be a 

large difference in the level of triglycerides between the low-carbohydrate 

group and the low-fat group. (Decrease in triglycerides is virtually the defi¬ 

nition of low-carbohydrate diets.) This expected difference was observed at 

the beginning of the trial, when the data were correlated with people actu¬ 

ally taking part in the experiment. With time more people dropped out, 

so there were fewer people on either diet and, in turn, there was decreased 

difference between the groups—not being on a low-carbohydrate diet 

is, unsurprisingly, similar to not being on a low-fat diet. The take-home 

message is that ITT and imputing data will reduce the real effect of the 

intervention. In a diet experiment, the nature of the diet and compliance 

might be related—if the diet is unpalatable, people might not stay on it— 

but you have to show this. “Might” is not data. Along these lines, however, 

it is likely that the major reinforcer, the major reason people will stay on a 

diet, is that it works. In any case, one cannot assume that the two are linked 

without specifically testing the idea. 



The Bottom Line 


Intention-to-treat comes from the realization that in some experiments, 

you don't know who followed the protocol and who didn't. In a clinic, you 

might write a prescription and not know if it's been filled. In those cases, 

you have no choice but to include everybody's performance. However, 



232 



Nutrition in Crisis 



the mechanical application of the idea in situations where you know who 

did or didn’t comply is another misguided and dogmatic application of 

statistics. ITT usually makes the better diet look worse than it actually is. 

Awareness of whether this method has been used is important for evaluat¬ 

ing a scientific publication. 



-CHAPTER 17- 



The Fiend 


That Lies Like Truth 



lpull in resolution and begin 

To doubt thequivocation of thefiend 

That lies like truth . 


—William Shakespeare,M^M 



E rrors, inappropriate use of statistics, and misleading presentations 

are everywhere in the medical literature, as IVe described in the 

previous chapters. Here I will summarize some of the specific 

problems weVe covered and try to provide you with a guide to what to 

look for in the medical literature, and especially in the popular media. 



Look for Visual Representations 


You have a right to demand, and the author of a scientific paper has an 

obligation to provide, a clear explanation of what the results of their study 

really mean. A good test of whether or not the authors are holding up 

their end of the arrangement is the number and clarity of the figures. 

Visual presentation is almost always stronger than long tabulation of 

numbers. This principle is simultaneously so reasonable, and at the same 

time so widely violated, that Howard Wainer wrote a whole book, Medical 

Illuminations , arguing that scientific papers need more figures instead of 

the dense tables that make results hard to understand and force the reader 

to take the authors' conclusions at face value. 1 


Scientific publication is changing, and increasingly, as more and more 

journals become open access and available online, results of scientific stud¬ 

ies will be universally available. An advantage to an open-access online 



234 



Nutrition in Crisis 



journal is that there is no longer a limit on pages or number of figures. Nor 

is there an extra charge to the authors for the use of color in these figures. 

It is unknown how frequently authors take advantage of these opportuni¬ 

ties, but the Golden Rule of Statistics remains the same: Let us see the 

data completely and clearly and in as many figures as it takes. 


Science, but Not Rocket Science 


“Eating breakfast reduces obesity” is not a principle from quantum elec¬ 

trodynamics. Most of us know whether or not eating breakfast makes us 

eat more or less during the day. Most of us also know that eating “good' 

anything is not good for losing weight—good might imply nutritious or 

tasty but usually means eating a lot. You don't need a physician, one who 

may have never studied nutrition, to tell you whether your perception is 

right or wrong. A degree in biochemistry is not required to understand 

the idea that adding sugar to your diet will increase your blood sugar—and 

the burden of proof is on anybody who wants to say that sugar is okay for 

people with diabetes. Anything is possible, but we start from what makes 

sense. Of course, there is technically sophisticated science and there are 

principles that require expertise to understand, but you should not auto¬ 

matically assume this is the case. 



Be Suspicious of Self-Serving Descriptions 


If a paper is about a diet that is described as “healthy,” your appropri¬ 

ate answer should be, “thats for me to decide.” The way the media tosses 

around the word healthy is bad enough, but in the context of a scientific 

paper it has to be considered an intellectual kiss of death. Nothing that you 

read after that can be taken at face value. If we knew what was healthy, we 

would not have an obesity epidemic and we would not need another paper 

to describe it. 


Similarly, guidelines, data, or analyses that the authors describe as 

“evidence-based medicine” are likely to be deeply flawed. To use the court 

of law as an analogy: There must be a judge to decide admissibility of 

evidence. You cant pat yourself on the back and expect to be considered 

impartial. The courts have ruled that testimony by experts has to make 



The Fiend That Lies Like Truth 



235 



sense, too. Credentials are not enough. As in the first principle, experts 

have to be able to explain data to the jury on the data’s own merit. Be 

suspicious if the authors tell you how many other people agree with their 

position. Science does not run on “majority rules” or consensus. 



Leaping Tall Buildings 


As indicated in chapter 12, the new grand principle of doing science is habeas 

corpus datorum —in other words, let’s see the body of the data. If the conclu¬ 

sion is counterintuitive and goes against previous work or common sense, 

then the data must be strong and clearly presented. Here’s another way to put 

it: If you say you can jump over a chair, I can cut you a lot of slack and assume 

you’re telling the truth. If you say you can jump over a building, however, I 

need to see you do it to be convinced. My daughter, at age nine, suggested 

an additional requirement. In a discussion of superheroes I pointed out that 

Superman used to be described as being “able to leap tall buildings in a single 

bound.” She pointed out that if you try to leap tall buildings, you only get 

a single bound. You can’t say your hundred-million-dollar, eight-year-long 

randomized controlled trial was not a fair test. The fat-cholesterol-heart 

hypothesis was sold as an absolute fact. None of the big clinical trials should 

have failed, but in the end, almost every single one did. 


To return to the idea of “Let’s see the body of the data”: Show me 

what was done before you start running it through the computer. Statistics 

might be important, but in a diet experiment where one has to assume 

that even a well-defined population is heterogeneous, you want to under¬ 

stand how the individuals performed. The compelling work of Nuttall and 

Gannon, 2 for example, showing that diabetes can be improved even in the 

absence of weight loss, is increased in impact by the presentation of the 

individual performance. Not only is there general good response in reduc¬ 

tion of blood glucose excursions, but all but two of the individual subjects 

benefited substantially, and all but one got at least somewhat better. 



Understand Observational Studies 


The usual warning offered by bloggers and others is that association 

does not imply causality, and that observational studies can only provide 



236 



Nutrition in Crisis 



hypotheses for future testing. A more accurate description, as detailed in 

chapter 13, is that observational studies do not necessarily imply causality. 

Sometimes they do and sometimes they don’t. The association between 

cigarette smoke and lung disease has a causal relation because the associa¬ 

tions are very strong and because the underlying reason for making the 

measurement was based on basic physiology, including the understanding 

of nicotine as a toxin. 


In this sense, observational studies test hypotheses rather than generat¬ 

ing them. There are an infinite number of phenomena, but when you try 

to make a specific comparison between two, you usually have an idea in 

mind, conscious or otherwise. When a study tries to find an association 

between egg consumption and diabetes, it is testing the hypothesis that 

eggs are a factor in the generation of diabetes. It might not be sensible or 

based on sound scientific principles, but its a hypothesis. If you do find 

a strong association with an unlikely hypothesis (this is the definition of 

intuition) then you have a new plausible hypothesis, and it must be further 

tested. It is important to question the hypothesis being tested in the first 

place, though. If the introduction section to the study says eggs have been 

associated with diabetes, you can at least check whether the reference is to 

an experimental study rather than a previous recommendation from some 

health agency. 


Meta-Analysis and the End of Science 


Doctors prefer a large study that is bad y to a small study that 


is good. 


—Anonymous 


While intention-to-treat is the most foolish activity plaguing modern 

medical literature, the meta-analysis is the most dangerous. In a meta¬ 

analysis you pool different studies to see if more information is available 

from the combination. As such it is inherently suspicious: If two studies 

have very different conditions, the results cannot sensibly be pooled. If, 

on the other hand, they are very similar, you have to ask why the results 

from the first study were not accepted. Finally, if the pooled studies give a 

different result than any of the individual studies, the authors are supposed 



The Fiend That Lies Like Truth 



237 



Group Statistics: 


Bill Gates Walks into a Bar . .. 


Everybody has their favorite example of how averages dont really 

tell you what you want to know, or how they are inappropriate 

for some situations. The reason most of these are funny is because 

they apply averages in cases where single events are important. 

One of my favorites, from the title of this sidebar: If Bill Gates 

walks into a bar, on average, everybody in the bar is a millionaire. 


Technically speaking, averages start with the assumption that 

there is a kind oPtrue’Value and that deviations are due to random 

error. If we could only control things well—if there were no wind 

resistance and each ball was absolutely uniform—all of the balls 

would always fall in the same place all the time. Any spread in 

values must be random rather than systematic. The familiar 

bell-shaped curve that illustrates the mean (average) value and 

deviations from the mean is referred to alternatively as the normal 

distribution or Gaussian distribution. Without going into details 

of statistics, the standard deviation, or SD, is supposed to give you 

a feel for how the real points deviate from the mean. From the SD, 

you can tell how reliable the data are. A low SD means that the 

mean value can be expected to be a reliable indication of what the 

actual data look Eke. A high SD, on the other hand, means that 

you can expect the data to be more spread out. 


There are many examples. The textbooks cite the blitz in 

London in World War II when the V-l “flying bombs” were 

distributed in a characteristic random pattern. (“Aiming” was 

presumed due to fixed launch sites and fixed fuel; deviations 

came from wind resistance and other random factors.) You can 

see a pretty good fit to the 3-D version of, in this case, the 

Poisson distribution. 


On the other hand, Allied bombing of the German town 

of Aachen destroyed much of the city while leaving intact the 



238 



Nutrition in Crisis 



famous cathedral where Charlemagne was crowned. The joke 

was that the cathedral was unharmed because that’s what 

we were aiming at It is generally assumed that what is good 

enough for bombing is good enough for social and biological 

science. But is it? Going from a large collection of individual 

points to two mathematical parameters (the mean and standard 

deviation), you obscure a good deal of information. A literally 

infinite number of different arrangements of points will give 

you the same mean and standard deviation. 


Even more importantly are you really sure that your data are 

uniform—that is, as the statisticians describe, that they come 

from the same population? Getting to the point: How are group 

statistics used in nutrition and medicine? The underlying prin¬ 

ciple is usually stated as the idea that "one size fits all.” But is 

this a good idea? When we see a spread in weight loss on differ¬ 

ent diets, for example, is that due to minor individual variations 

(one subject always goes to the gym right after breakfast), or is 

it due to fundamental differences between people? For example, 

might there be two classes of people (Eg., insulin-sensitive and 

insulin - resistant)? 


Nobody Loses an Average Amount of Weight 

Use of averages depends on uniformity in the population. Yet 

in diet, we don’t really believe that. Calling attention to all the 

people who eat more than you do and gain less weight than you 

do is considered an anecdotal observation. (The fact that you 

don’t want to believe it adds some credibility.) There have been 

experiments, however. David Allisons group 3 studied a number 

of identical twins, putting them all on a weight-reduction diet 

for a month and measuring how much weight they lost. They 

calculated an energy deficit (differences between calories in and 

calories out) and compared that to the actual energy deficit 

(weight loss minus food intake). 



The Fiend That Lies Like Truth 



239 




Estimated Energy Deficit JMJ] 


Figure 17.1. Relationship of estimated and measured energy deficit for 

fourteen twin pairs (same numbers]. The dotted line is the regression 

of estimate on measured energy deficit and the solid line represents 

identity (measured deficit « estimated). Subjects above the solid line 

(of identity) are absolutely less efficient than those below the line. Data 

from V Rainer et at, '‘A Twin Study of Weight Loss and Metabolic Efficiency” 

International Journal of Obesity and Related Metabohc Disorders 25, no 4 

(2001): 533-537. 


The pairs of twins in figure 17.1 were similar in their energy 

deficit, but there were major variations between twins. The 

study was carried out in a hospital but, given the possible 

random variations of twenty-eight people over the course of a 

month, this is pretty good evidence for what is popularly called 

“metabolic advantage” (people below the solid line) or maybe 

metabolic “disadvantage” (people above). 




240 



Nutrition in Crisis 



to point out what the original study did wrong (and the original authors 

are supposed to agree). 


Simply adding more subjects is not considered a guarantee of reliability; 

however, papers in the medical literature frequently cite their large number 

of subjects as one of their strengths. If a meta-analysis is good for anything 

(which is questionable), it is for its originally intended role of evaluating 

small, underpowered studies with the hope that putting them together 

might reveal something you didn't originally see. It is a kind of “Hail Mary” 

last-ditch play. It was not intended for appropriately powered studies with 

a large number of subjects. 


One of the benefits, conscious or unconscious, that keeps meta-analysis 

going is that it is perfect for current medical research. With meta-analysis, 

no experiment ever fails and no principle is ever disproved. Sugar causes 

heart attacks, cholesterol causes heart attacks, red meat causes heart 

attacks, and statins prevent heart attacks—it doesn't matter how many 

studies show no effect. One winner and you can do a meta-analysis. Just 

one more expensive trial and we'll show that saturated fat is bad. Plus you 

don’t even have to explain what the previous researcher did wrong, as you 

might in a real experiment. 


The idea that simply adding more subjects will improve reliability is not 

reasonable. Most of us think that if a phenomenon has large variability, 

then mixing studies will reduce predictability although it might sharpen 

statistical significance. And again, in science it is expected that if your 

results contradict previous experiments, you will provide evidence as to the 

cause of the differences. What did previous investigators do wrong? Do 

those investigators now agree that you've improved things? Probably not, 

especially if you don't ask them. 



Be Suspicious of Grand Principles 


“Randomized controlled trials are the gold standard.” “Metabolic ward 

studies are the gold standard.” “Observational studies are only good for 

generating hypotheses.” Such grand principles do not play a part in the 

physical sciences, where the method that we choose depends on the ques¬ 

tion that we want to answer. You do not need to carry out a long-term trial 

to see if a treatment is appropriate for an acute condition. You do not need 



The Fiend That Lies Like Truth 



241 



Saturated Fatty Acids, 

Carbohydrates, and Meta-Analysis 


A number of important meta-analyses have examined the effect 

of saturated fatty acids on cardiovascular risk. They all show a 

very small benefit if the saturated fatty acids are replaced with 

carbohydrate. Examination of the data, however, reveals that 

almost all of the included studies failed to show any significant 

effect of replacing saturated fat, and yet the authors came up 

with an answer* How is this possible? It is the consequence of 

group-think* If everybody—the editors, the reviewers, and ulti¬ 

mately the reader—assumes that meta-analysis is an acceptable 

method, then peer review will be meaningless. 



a randomized controlled study to find out whether penicillin is effective for 

treating gram-positive infections. Penicillin is a drug and you might have 

to do a randomized controlled trial to determine safety, but at the efficacy 

end, if the results are clear-cut, only a small number of tests are necessary. 

How many? It depends on how many people recover spontaneously. A 

statistician can figure this out for you if you ask the question appropriately. 


The idea of the randomized controlled trial as a gold standard has really 

never recovered from Smith and Pells landmark paper, “Parachute Use 

to Prevent Death and Major Trauma Related to Gravitational Challenge: 

Systematic Review of Randomised Controlled Trials.” 4 In the paper, Smith 

and Pell concluded that “the effectiveness of parachutes has not been 

subjected to rigorous evaluation by using randomised controlled trials.” 


Described as both funny and profound, the paper included all the relevant 

information, making fun of the excessive statistical detail in the literature: 

“We chose the Mantel-Haenszel test to assess heterogeneity, and sensitivity 

and subgroup analyses and fixed effects weighted regression techniques to 

explore causes of heterogeneity. We selected a funnel plot to assess publica¬ 

tion bias visually and Egger s and B egg’s tests to test it quantitatively.” 



242 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Smith and Pell pointed out that there were only two solutions to the 

problem of a lack of randomized controlled trials: “The first is that we accept 

that, under exceptional circumstances, common sense might be applied when 

considering the potential risks and benefits of interventions. The second is 

that we continue our quest for the holy grail of exclusively evidence-based 

interventions and preclude parachute use outside the context of a properly 

conducted trial.” (Emphasis added) 


In the end, the authors suggested that “those who advocate evidence- 

based medicine and criticise use of interventions that lack an evidence 

base ... demonstrate their commitment by volunteering for a double blind, 

randomised, placebo controlled, crossover trial.” 


The bottom line is that science has to make sense and does not depend 

on arbitrary rules. Smith and Pell made fun of how often authors attempt 

to snow us. There really are statistical tests with those names, but science 

is not about fixed rules and accounting. It is about understanding each 

experiment and knowing the question to be asked. 


The faults that plague the medical literature have made it a minefield for 

readers, and more often than not, readers will not be happy with the latest 

study showing that processed food or one or another of the usual suspects 

will kill you. Armed with a few principles, you can understand what is 

explicitly wrong with these studies that so strongly violate common sense. 

Failure to show individual data; the use of questionable practices, such as 

intention-to-treat and meta-analyses; and, most of all, failure to actually 

apply common sense, are the things to look for. It will be hard to accept that 

the Harvard School of Public Health, all the best and the brightest, could 

be making elementary mistakes, but the proof and precedents are there. 



PART 5 



The Second 

Low-Carbohydrate 

Revolution 




-CHAPTER 18- 



Nutrition in Crisis 



M ore work needs to be done” is the standard conclusion of weak 

dietary studies or those that got the “wrong answer.” As in any 

biological science, what we know is much less than what we 

don't know. When it comes to moving forward on the low-carbohydrate 

argument, however, no more work needs to be done: Switching from your 

current diet to a low-carbohydrate or ketogenic diet is usually the healthi¬ 

est change you can make. We know this because it has been put to the test. 

After being subjected to forty years of persistent criticism—some rigorous, 

some preposterous and outlandish, some virulent—carbohydrate restric¬ 

tion has proven to be the most effective way to lose weight and the first 

line of treatment for diabetes. 


Yet for forty years, the medical and nutritional establishment have pulled 

out all the stops in a compulsive effort to find something medically wrong 

with low-carbohydrate diets. They've come up with nothing at all. In head- 

to-head comparisons, regardless of the length of the trials, low-carbohydrate 

almost always wins. At the same time, we have forty years of multicenter, 

multimillion-dollar trials that fail to show any risk from total fat or saturated 

fat or dietary cholesterol. We need to face—and capitalize on—the scientific 

data that stand clearly before us before any more work needs to be done. 


For the individual dieter, we have found the optimal strategy. It's not a 

magic bullet, and it's not something that works the same for everybody, but 

it's the best we have. The challenge now is to work out a plan to implement 

the low-carbohydrate strategy. The crisis in nutrition is that you might be 

on your own. Your physician or health provider is not guaranteed to be on 

your side and might, in fact, be trying to dissuade you from the best course 

of action. If you're lucky, and your provider is reasonable, you might be 

able to teach them. A physician, in particular, might have no training at 

all in nutrition, and the practice of analyzing original research critically is 



246 



Nutrition in Crisis 



unlikely to be part of their daily activity, so you can point them to relevant 

literature. Nutrition in Crisis would be a good start. 


Twilight of the Lipophobes 


So how close are we to good science and good guidance on nutrition? I 

spoke at the LEO Conference, held in Gothenburg, Sweden, which cele¬ 

brates nonconformity in science. The 2008 meeting honored Uffe Ravnskov, 

the arch cholesterol skeptic. 1 Quoting Max Planck, speakers before me 

suggested that if you really wanted to introduce a new idea in science, you 

had to wait for the old generation to die out. When I spoke, 2 I suggested 

that since I was in that generation, we might want to do it a little sooner. 


How far do we have to go? As in any highly contentious field, the answer 

you get depends on whom you ask. While low-fat still hangs over everything, 

there are now numerous disclaimers: “Only saturated fat is a risk” or maybe 

it s only a problem if “you replace polyunsaturated fat with saturated fat.” 

The large, expensive, randomized controlled trials that successfully showed no 

effect of dietary total saturated fat or cholesterol are still assumed to have never 

taken place—and although the case for low-carbohydrate and its benefits is 

established, the best that can be done according to many is to claim that “it 

is not any better” than low-fat diets, despite the fact that low-fat never wins, 

and only ever draws. Attacking sugar is considered a first step—presumably 

because sugar is an easy target. Whether the second step, if there really is one, 

is a more comprehensive meaningful low-carbohydrate recommendation, is 

never stated. It is a political strategy that involves fundamental disdain for the 

audience, and one that is almost guaranteed to backfire. 


Things are progressing, yes. Many researchers and organizations, while 

admitting no wrongdoing, are slowly giving in to low-carbohydrate. Yet 

there is simultaneously unrestrained backlash, which has taken a very 

ugly turn toward personal and professional attacks on low-carbohydrate 

researchers and journalists. 



Quashing Dissent around the World 


Coincidentally, it was in Sweden that the medical establishment revealed 

its resistance to criticism and to new ideas. Dr. Annika Dahlqvist lives in 



Nutrition in Crisis 



247 



Njurunda, Sundsvall. She described on her blog 3 how she discovered that 

a low-carbohydrate diet would help in her own battle with obesity and 

various health problems that included enteritis (irritable bowel syndrome, 

or IBS), gastritis, fibromyalgia, chronic fatigue syndrome, insomnia, and 

snoring. Recommending low-carbohydrate diets for her patients and 

publicly advertising her ideas drew a certain amount of media attention, 

leading to a run-in with the authorities. In November 2006 she lost her 

job at Njurunda Medical Center. She was ultimately exonerated in January 

2008, when the National Board of Health and Welfare found that a low- 

carbohydrate diet was “consistent with good clinical practice.”This was the 

likely prelude to the announcement in 2013 that the Swedish Agency for 

Health Technology Assessment, which is charged with assessing national 

health care treatment, endorsed low-carbohydrate diets for weight loss. 

While their statements were far from enthusiastic, it was one of a number 

of events that indicated the fall of the low-fat paradigm and the no-holds- 

barred backlash of nutritional medicine. 


The most bizarre case, which was resolved as I was finishing this 

book, occurred in Australia. An orthopedic surgeon, Dr. Gary Fettke, 

had become the target of the Australian Health Practitioner Regulation 

Agency (AHPRA) after he publically discussed the benefits of a low-carb 

diet. In a letter dated November 1,2016, the AHPRA told Fettke, “There 

is nothing associated with your medical training or education that makes 

you an expert or authority in the field of nutrition, diabetes or cancer.” 4 

The ensuing persecution was remarkably unprofessional and ugly, and it 

wasn t until nearly two years later that the AHPRA exonerated Fettke and 

offered a belated and inadequate apology. The training of any physician in 

nutrition is, in my view, problematic, but it has been rightly pointed out, 

in response to the original letter, that Fettke received the same training as 

other doctors. 


The letter continued: “Even if, in the future, your views on the benefits of 

the [low-carb, high-fat] LCHF lifestyle become the accepted best medical 

practice, this does not change the fundamental fact that you are not suit¬ 

ably trained or educated as a medical practitioner to be providing advice 

or recommendations on this topic as a medical practitioner. ”In short, even 

if the AHPRA recognized the truth, they would not defend Fettke s right 

to say it. As many noted at the time, Dr. Fettke is in fact trained to remove 



248 



Nutrition in Crisis 



the limbs of those whose diabetes has reached a critical point, despite the 

AHPRA’s attempts to diminish his credentials. This lack of due process 

and common decency speaks to the need for some kind of regulation. The 

issue hasn’t receive significant coverage in traditional media, which have 

kept it relatively invisible, but the recent turn of events may bring out just 

how bad things are. The fallout from this scandal is just being uncovered, 

and Marika Sboros’s blog, foodmed.net, is a likely source for the unfolding 

story. Her recent blog post titled “Fettke: Cover-Up Grows After Case 

Against Him Collapses?” is both gripping and intensely distressing. 


Stateside, a North Carolina blogger named Steve Cooksey was enjoined 

from recommending a low-carbohydrate diet for diabetes by the North 

Carolina Board of Dietetics/Nutrition. After a near-death experience from 

his own diabetes, and after experiencing the benefits of carbohydrate restric¬ 

tion, he felt it might be good to share the information with others, with due 

disclaimers about not offering medical advice. That was too much for the 

professional nutritionists who somehow have gotten the state legislature to 

designate them as sole purveyors of nutritional advice. Enlisting the aid of 

the Institute for Justice, Cooksey won a First Amendment lawsuit and the 

North Carolina board realized that it could not tell American citizens what 

they could and could not say. The board changed its guidelines, but the fury 

of a nutritionist spurned is great—and even worse down under. 


The Dietitians Association of Australia (DAA) registers dietitians and 

essentially controls whether they can do their jobs in hospitals, universi¬ 

ties, or private practice. In 2015, the DAA expelled Jennifer Elliott, a 

dietitian with thirty years’ experience and a highly regarded book for 

patients. She had also published a peer-reviewed critique of the diet-heart 

hypothesis, which likely contributed to the DAA action. The complaint 

from a DAA-registered dietitian was that Elliott’s recommendation 

of low-carbohydrate diets for her patients with type 2 diabetes was 

not “evidence-based.” The DAA upheld the complaint. In my personal 

communication with Claire Hewat, head of the DAA, however, she claimed 

that the cause of dismissal had not been the recommendations of low- 

carbohydrate diet but something else that could not be revealed because it 

was confidential. The power of low-carbohydrate diets to provoke hostile 

behavior remains fascinating if incomprehensible. In a blog post on the 

subject, I made an analogy to the Protestant Reformation, although unlike 




Nutrition in Crisis 



249 



The Tim Noakes Case 


It is troublesome to hear that Tim is being attacked 

so strongly for what seems to be a trivial matter ; 

when there are plenty of real problems in health 

care and our world more broadly . 


—Walter Willett 5 


Most bizarre for its virulence remains the determination with 

which the Health Professionals Council of South Africa 

(HPSCA) tried to maintain control by attacking Tim Noakes. 

A retired physician and widely admired sports expert, and only a 

relatively recent spokesman for carbohydrate restriction, Noakes 

answered a comment on Twitter that suggested a newborn 

could be weaned to a low-carbohydrate diet, not particularly 

different from the recommendations of Australian authorities. 

Yes, this is about a single tweet, which wasn't even taken into 

account by the individual who J d asked the question in first place. 

A nutritionist was insulted by the affair and brought the case 

to the HPSCA. The question become whether Noakes s tweet 

constituted medical advice—the primary 7 affirmative argument 

being that because he was a medical doctor, anything Noakes 

said could be considered advice. This is just as ridiculous as the 

folk myth that a black belt in karate has to register his hands at 

local police stations as lethal weapons. 


The HPSCA came down hard on Noakes but, after two 

years of trial, he was exonerated. The HPCSAs own committee 

found that no harm derived from the tweet, and that his advice 

was “evidence-based” and within normal standards. HPSCA 

was able to establish a warning for other medical profession¬ 

als, since as Noakes explained, “It s been very, very demanding 

on us and on our lives and financially it's been huge.” Noakes 

asked, further, “Did they ever consider the consequences for my 



250 



Nutrition in Crisis 



wife and myself and our family?” Indicating that it did not, the 

HPSCA immediately appealed the decision of its own commit¬ 

tee. While the decision of the new body was supposed to be 

produced with all deliberate speed, innate pettiness presumably 

caused them to drag it out for months. Noakes was finally exon¬ 

erated. Besides being a person of statute in sports medicine, 

he is a charming, friendly person—I met him at a landmark 

conference at Ohio State University just as the final edits on 

this book were prepared. 



Luther Jennifer Elliott was not particularly rebellious and was not trying 

to reform anything except her patients’ dietary habits. The DAA func¬ 

tions, after all, as a professional dietitians’ organization and should, as in 

Macbeth , against the murder shut the door, not bear the knife itself. There 

was some press coverage but little public awareness or professional outcry 

over the lack of natural justice and common decency. 


Dissent will not be tolerated. When Nina Teicholz, author of The Big 

Fat Surprise, published a critical analysis in the BMJ describing excesses 

of the expert report by the HHS-USDA Dietary Guidelines Advisory 

Committee (DGAC)—notably that it did not even discuss low-carbo¬ 

hydrate diets—Michael Jacobson, the obsessive and humorless leader of 

the Center for Science in the Public Interest (CSPI, a largely vegetarian 

watchdog group), initiated a petition to have the paper retracted because 

it was “so riddled with errors.” The alleged errors turned out to be less 

about Teicholz’s scientific conclusions than about who used what database 

as evidence. From Jacobson’s petition: “Teicholz states that ‘in the NEL 

systematic review on saturated fats from 2010 ... fewer than 12 small trials 

are cited, and none supports the hypothesis that saturated fats cause heart 


disease_’ Correction: It is incorrect to state that none of the trials cited 


in the 2010 NEL review supports the hypothesis.” 


The paper was not retracted, presumably because no scientific issue 

was raised and Teicholz’s critique was judged “within the realm of 



Nutrition in Crisis 



251 



scientific debate.” A revealing bit of information was exposed along 

the way, however: Frank Hu of Harvard School of Public Health, 

lead author of the DGAC review of saturated fats, personally sent out 

multiple emails to colleagues, putting together a posse of more than 

150 people, including many graduate students, who ultimately signed 

the CSPI petition. The pjetition remains problematic in that it did not 

represent an assembly of people who had expressed concern, but rather 

an assortment of people who were contacted by Dr. Hu with an unsolic¬ 

ited request to sign the petition. 



The Big Picture 


The extent to which repressive behavior actually reduces research on or 

acceptance of dietary carbohydrate restriction is unknown. However, the 

hostile, personal attacks—generally uncharacteristic of scientific behav¬ 

ior—represent desperation. If they could actually deal with the issue on a 

scientific basis, they wouldn't have to behave like this. It is likely, however, 

that the aggression is a response to seeing people, including their patients, 

find out the answer for themselves. 


The studies from Volek's lab are particularly compelling, but in fact, almost 

every comparison in the scientific literature shows that low-carbohydrate 

diets are more effective for weight loss and most other metabolic distur¬ 

bances. The rationale is that dietary carbohydrate is the major stimulus for 

secretion of the anabolic hormone insulin. Chapter 9 explained how it is 

possible to lose more weight, calorie for calorie, on a low-carbohydrate 

diet: The bottom line is that insulin slows the breakdown (lipolysis) of fat, 

and if you consume another meal before the system has had a chance to 

deal with stored fat, it will accumulate. Also, on a low-carbohydrate diet, 

your appetite goes down—satiety is increased, so most people find it easier 

to adhere to a carbohydrate-restricted diet than to any other. 


The real win for low-carbohydrate diets is personal: It changes your 

interaction with food. For people with a weight problem, every meal is a 

battle. Every time you sit down at the table you're trying to make sure you 

don't cross some line of calories, fat, or whatever other metric you're keep¬ 

ing an eye on. If you make a substantial cut in the amount of carbohydrates 

that you eat, even if you are not overly precise, even if you spend a couple of 



252 



Nutrition in Crisis 



days at the conference buffet, you are unlikely to gain any weight. You lose 

a substantial part of your obsession with and anxiety about food. 


It is reassuring, too, to know that we don’t have a biological need for 

carbohydrate and that the opposite of lowering fat intake is not eating all 

the fat in sight. It might well be true, as critics of low-carbohydrate diet 

say, that the recommended low-carbohydrate strategy includes assurances 

that “you can eat all the fat that you want,” but the emphasis is on want. 

Fat is filling. How much do you want? It is important to stress that the 

low-fat idea was not originally instituted to deal with obesity but rather 

to prevent heart disease (which it didn’t) and it always had a moralistic 

overtone. It was somebody’s idea of what we should have been eating 

during the millennia in which haute cuisine evolved and people perfected 

sausages and other food in ethnic cuisines. The low-fat idea was the work 

of Puritans. It gave rise to the obesity, epidemic, and the fact that many 

people did not get fat is only a testament to the adaptability of humans in 

dealing with all kinds of food. 


The real impact of low-carbohydrate and ketogenic diets is the tie to 

biologic mechanism and the generality of control of metabolism by insulin 

and associated hormones. From a scientific standpoint, the main ideas are 

established, and all that remains is political and social acceptance. As this 

acceptance sets in, our understanding of metabolic control mechanisms 

might unlock new and exciting possibilities. 



-CHAPTER 19- 



Cancer 


A New Frontier for Low-Carbohydrate 



I t was in July 2012 that I realized we'd won. It was now clear that we 

had a consistent set of scientific ideas supporting the central role of 

insulin signaling in basic biochemistry. We could see a continuum 

with the effectiveness of dietary carbohydrate restriction for obesity, diabe¬ 

tes, and general health. The practical considerations—how much to eat of 

this, how much to eat of that—hadn't yet been fully resolved, but we had 

the kernel of a scientific principle. In fact, it was not so much that we had 

the answer as that we had the right question. We were, at last, looking at 

diet, metabolism, and disease as a single entity. We were asking whether 

pathologic changes could be precisely traced back to disease, and that 

meant focusing on the downstream effects of high-carbohydrate intake. 

In science, the question is frequently more important than the answer. If 

you know what to look for, you are more likely to find it. Of course it 

wasn't originally about winning, or even fighting. When my colleagues 

and I got into this about ten years earlier, coming from a background of 

basic biochemistry, we hadn't anticipated that it would be such a battle, 

that there would be so much resistance to what we thought was normal 

scientific practice. Surprisingly, it was cancer studies that made clear how it 

all fit together, and seemed to signal the success of all our work. 


We generally recognize cancer as a genetic disease—not necessarily 

inherited, but a disease associated with genetic mutations. A great deal of 

research has gone into identifying the mutations and where the products 

of the mutant genes fit in. While this has given rise to much valuable 

information, it has been impossible to identify a single mechanism for 

cancer. Numerous mutations have been identified but finding a common 

thread has rarely been possible—and the information we do have has led 



254 



Nutrition in Crisis 



to only a small number strategies, pharmaceutical or otherwise, for treat¬ 

ment or prevention. 


The focus of cancer research, however, has recently begun to turn 

back to earlier studies on the relation of energy metabolism to the 

cancerous state. With this shift in focus, researchers have paid greater 

attention to the Warburg effect, the observation that many tumors rely on 

glycolysis—the anaerobic processing of glucose to pyruvic acid described 

in chapter 5. The re-appreciation of energy metabolism has had wide- 

ranging consequence. This is not to say that the genetic mutation theory 

has somehow been replaced by a metabolic theory. In fact, it is usually 

impossible to separate genetic and metabolic effects. Metabolism runs 

on enzymes, which are proteins, and proteins are primarily controlled by 

their synthesis, the gene products. In other words, although you could 

consider DNA to be a blueprint, that blueprint is not simply copied and 

sent out to the factory. Rather, we keep going back to the blueprint, and 

the workers vote on whether the body will work on one or another part of 

the blueprint, and choose what to do with each piece created. This slightly 

strained analogy means that the total state of the cell, its energy profile 

and cell constituents, will determine which proteins stipulated by the 

genome will be expressed and the extent to which they will be active. The 

focus on metabolism was the thread that brought us back to nutrition and 

the control of energy production and utilization. There suddenly seemed 

to be better appreciation of the need for a nutritional approach. I felt that 

we had reached some kind of turning point. 


The locale was a conference in Washington, DC, called “Metabolism, 

Diet and Disease.” It might have been better billed as “Metabolism, Drugs 

and Cancers,” at least based on the first day, but there was a surprising 

thread in the various talks—surprising to me, at least—about the very 

strong association between obesity and risk of cancer. Less surprising 

to me, because of the numerous studies I’d read, were the presentations 

about the effect of total dietary calorie restriction, the apparent basis for 

the connection between obesity and cancer: Reducing calories has been 

shown to reliably increase longevity and to control cancer in animals. An 

additional element of the conversation was the hormone insulin, which 

had popped up as a major player in various experiments on cancer. Then 

there was the identification of downstream signaling elements—the 



Cancer 



255 



compounds and proteins that transmit the information about cell stimu¬ 

lation to the interior of the cell. These were important results, and again, 

they frequently pointed to the components of insulin pathways and even 

to an association between cancer and diabetes. This last point was not 

entirely new to me: Outstanding experiments had indicated the critical 

role of insulin, and I always wondered why the connection to dietary 

carbohydrate hadn't been made. In any case, the second day of the confer¬ 

ence included presentations on dietary carbohydrate restriction, and 

although he was not listed on the organizing committee, Gary Taubes had 

helped set up the event and deserves credit for bringing together cancer 

people and low-carbohydrate people. 


My colleague Dr. Eugene Fine presented a poster at the Washington 

conference. Many conferences have poster sessions: Presenters pin posters, 

typically four by six feet, to easels, and attendees at the conference can then 

discuss the subject matter with the presenter. Posters don't always have a 

big impact, so we were grateful to Gary Taubes for making Eugene’s poster 

known to the main speakers.The paper on which the poster was based, now 

published in the journal Nutrition , a describes a small study conducted with 

ten seriously ill cancer patients. The study had the modest goal of show¬ 

ing that a ketogenic diet was a safe and feasible regimen, and in fact, the 

patients did well. Six of the ten stabilized or went into partial remission. 

By itself, this study would be considered only a small step forward, but in 

fact, it was key in tying together the fields of carbohydrate restriction and 

cell signaling in the context of normal and cancer cells. The experiment 

was difficult to do:The patients had to have refused or failed chemotherapy 

and exhausted all the traditional regimens. Yet it was incredibly significant. 

Given what we know about insulin and low-carbohydrate diets, the experi¬ 

ment should have been done twenty years ago. These two lines of thought 

simply had to be brought together. 


In hindsight, it seems that workers in carbohydrate restriction should 

have paid more attention to downstream cell signaling. We thought that 

the role of insulin in system biochemistry made it clear that carbohydrate 

restriction was built on a solid foundation. The resistance to the idea seemed 

incomprehensible and parochial, but this conference made it obvious that 

more was needed to form a consistent biological story. It became clear that 

there was a conceptual barrier to acceptance of carbohydrate restriction 



256 



Nutrition in Crisis 



beyond the traditional resistance to anything associated with the Atkins 

diet. There were additional factors. There was a mind-set that prevented 

adequate synthesis of all the information. 



Targeting Cancer through Insulin Inhibition 


When approaching cancer, much of the valuable work in cell biology tended 

to downplay the biochemistry at the upstream-stimulus level—namely, 

what you eat and its immediate biologic consequences. The major goal had 

been to characterize the individual components in the inner working of 

the cell and to search for those components that were specifically malfunc¬ 

tioning in pathological states. These agents—primarily proteins and the 

associated genes from which they were expressed—could then be targeted 

with drugs, either directly or indirectly at the genomic level. With the 

important observation that calorie restriction could ameliorate or prevent 

cancer, a link with obesity was established, and in some way excess calories 

became interchangeable with weight gain. The obesity-cancer link became 

a serial link and it was assumed—as it is frequently assumed in nutrition 

that preventing obesity was part of preventing associated pathologies. As 

a result of this framing, successes of carbohydrate restriction in improving 

cardiovascular risk factors, for example, have frequently been dismissed 

as due to the attendant weight loss. This, despite the evidence that the 

improvements in risk factors or other outcomes persist even in the absence 

of weight loss, or even when benefits were demonstrated in eucaloric trials. 


The rationale of the research, then, was that calorie restriction—the 

recognized approach to obesity—would point to those intracellular 

components that could be targeted for drug development. In many cases 

this was a conceptual error. In nutrition it is likely that the doctrine of “a 

calorie is a calorie” is the single greatest impediment to understanding. 

Otherwise sophisticated and informative experiments were compromised 

by the identification of obesity with excess calories, and the failure to look 

beyond this effect—by the failure to ask how the separate nutrients, carbo¬ 

hydrate, fat, and protein, individually affect cellular metabolism, and how 

these effects interact (there is, after all, no calorie receptor). Similarly, as I 

mentioned above, I and other workers in carbohydrate restriction failed to 

see how important it was to look at downstream cellular signaling. 



Cancer 



257 



Fines study treated ten seriously ill cancer patients with low- 

carbohydrate ketogenic diets and showed that it was a safe and feasible 

regimen for such patients.The rationale followed from the fact that rapidly 

growing tumors have a requirement for glucose, but simply reducing 

carbohydrates to give the host an advantage was unlikely to be effective 

because blood glucose is regulated to stay fairly constant and the cancers 

are good at getting whatever glucose is there. Tumors overexpress GLUTl 

protein, the non-insulin-dependent glucose receptors. The odds were 

therefore strongly stacked against us when it came to effectively depriv¬ 

ing the cancer of glucose. The question was whether ketone bodies, the 

specific consequence of very low-carbohydrate intake, might themselves 

exert control over the cancer cells. 


Ketosis, the state associated with very low-energy or very low- 

carbohydrate intake, held some promise. Fine hypothesized that if we 

think of cancer in terms of genetics, we could think of cancer cells as 

having evolved throughout the life of the individual, an individual whose 

systemic environment, in a modern setting, would be unlikely to experi¬ 

ence any significant level of ketosis—and that would therefore be unlikely 

to provide any selective pressure for the adaptation to use of ketone bodies 

as a fuel source. The host, on the other hand, was well adapted to this 

substrate: It is unlikely that our ancestors regularly had three square meals 

a day. Some fraction of cancer lines, then, might not deal well with a 

ketotic environment. 


In the experiment it turned out that those patients who became stable 

or showed partial remission had the highest level of ketone bodies. Figure 

19 .\A shows individual time points with different symbols for each 

patient. As expected, there was a correlation between ketone bodies and 

insulin levels. 


Although this was a small sample, figure 19.15 shows that while some 

of the patients had progressive disease, some became stable or showed 

partial remission. The level of ketone bodies was the best predictor of 

this outcome. Figure 19.1 C shows, on the other hand, that improvements 

did not depend on calories or weight loss. Patients who demonstrated 

stable disease or partial remission had a threefold higher average ketotic 

response compared to those with continued progressive disease. However, 

both groups showed similar calorie deficits, or, as shown, degree of weight 



p-OH-butyrate (diet/baseline] TO p-OH-butyrate (diet/baseline) 



258 



Nutrition in Crisis 



40' 


35 T 


25 


20 



15 


10 




0.2 



0.4 



0.6 0.8 1.0 


[insulin] (diet/baseline) 



1.2 



1.4 




Stable Disease/ Progressive 

Partial Remission Disease 



70 



g 60 



Cl 


(/) 


01 


SI 


l/> 




50 



a> 40 



30 


20 




CT3 



£ 10 



Stable Disease/ Progressive 


Partial Remission Disease 



Figure 19.1. A f Ketonemia versus insulinemia: The lowest insulinemia correlated with 

the highest ketonemia levels. Different symbols represent values for each patient. 

B, Ketosis versus disease progression: Patients who demonstrated stable disease or 

partial remission had much greater ketotic response compared to those with progres¬ 

sive disease. C, Calorie deficit versus outcome: The stable disease/partial remission 

and progressive disease groups showed no difference in calorie reduction. 







































Cancer 



259 



loss—suggesting that the well-established benefits in caloric restriction 

reflect an underlying mechanism beyond the energy itself. 



Nailing the Link 


A small study in advanced cancer patients might be considered only 

a minor step forward, but in fact it ties into a vast area of research on 

the downstream signaling in both cancer and normal cells—that is, the 

changes in the cell following stimulation by an external food or hormones. 

Of particular interest is the well-known effect of calorie restriction. It was 

widely understood that dietary calorie restriction would have a therapeutic 

effect on animals with cancer, and in addition, that reducing calories was 

the only way to prolonglife in animals. Studies following this idea showed 

that it was an insulin pathway that was involved. Eugene Fines study 

nailed the link for us. It is an encouraging situation, because if this is the 

worst of times in nutrition, it is also the golden age of biology. Bringing all 

of the science to bear on the problem has great promise indeed. 




-CHAPTER 20- 



The Future of Nutrition 



T he low-carbohydrate revolution of 2002 was precipitated by 

the popular exposes on the low-fat-diet-heart hypothesis and 

its consistent failure to find support in large clinical trials. The 

well-armed forces mustered by the diet-heart hypothesis fared poorly in 

the early confrontations with the actual experimental evidence. It was a 

case of failure to accept failure, even as authorities tried to generalize the 

proposed harm of dietary fat to obesity and diabetes. Scientific devel¬ 

opments, however, have continued to reinforce the idea that control of 

metabolism by insulin and other hormones is the key factor in weight loss, 

diabetes, metabolic syndrome, and—now—perhaps even cancer. Classic, 

well-controlled experiments have nailed the idea: Carbohydrate restriction 

is the best therapy for all of the features of metabolic syndrome and the 

default treatment for diabetes, consistent with its nature as a disease of 

carbohydrate intolerance. 


At the same time, underwhelming tests of the diet-heart hypothesis 

continue to be conducted, despite the base of evidence to the contrary. 

Support comes from meta-analyses and epidemiological statistical juggling, 

whose minimal positive outcomes—again, an odds ratio of 1.5 seems even 

worse if described as 60:40—are just as damning as the well-documented 

failures. Bias against publishing studies of low-carbohydrate diets seems as 

strong as ever. A low-carbohydrate paper submitted to the New England 

Journal of Medicine, the BMJ, or other major journals will be treated with 

superficial politeness if you re lucky, or—more likely—palpable disdain. 


For scientists and consumers alike, the situation has become more 

confusing, more ambiguous. A major factor appears to be a near disin¬ 

tegration of standards. Epidemiologic studies trying to show the risks in 

saturated fat (good fats-bad fats), in red meat, in sugar (good carbs-bad 

carbs) continue to proliferate. In the absence of critical peer review, they 



262 



Nutrition in Crisis 



are accepted for publication, and their conclusions are presented at face 

value in the media, and notably in the online medical publications from 

which physicians receive much of their information. The same journals 

and popular medical sites publish papers describing how bad the obesity 

epidemic is, and directly, or by implication, blame the patient. Ironically 

these journals also publish articles lamenting the very breakdown in stan¬ 

dards in the medical literature to which they are the primary contributors. 1 


The effects of carbohydrate per se are generally ignored. If they are 

mentioned at all, it is always to point out the dangers of “refined” carbohy¬ 

drate. Wild, exaggerated dangers are attributed to sugar and high-fructose 

corn syrup, which are misguidedly separated from carbohydrates at large, 

and the lipophobes seem to have maintained control of the market: 

Low-fat versions of nearly every product, even “half and half,” are pushed 

in the supermarkets. Yet, at the same time, there is a sense that it is all over. 

The accumulating failures of low-fat and the popular books and articles 

exposing those failures are finally taking their toll. The publication in 2014 

of Nina Teicholz’s The Big Fat Surprise might well be the second low- 

carbohydrate revolution’s Common Sense, but whatever ultimately deals 

the death blow, it is clear that the nutritional establishment has cracked. 

Squabbling among the proponents of low-fat is a sure sign. 


What’s Next? 


It is, perhaps, the end of the beginning. 


—Winston Churchill 


It is clear that low-fat is dead. Burying it seems like a good idea. Continual 

failures of large trials, in combination with the success of alternative 

approaches, especially control of metabolism through the glucose-insulin 

axis, makes a new method of thinking inevitable. 


Surprisingly, the key scientific focus of the second low-carbohydrate 

revolution might be cancer. It has long been recognized that total caloric 

restriction, at least in animals, is of benefit in slowing progression of 

disease and extending life. This has led to the questionable paradigm 

of identifying calories with obesity and identifying obesity, in turn, as a 

stimulus for pathological effects. One problem is that calorie restriction, 



The Future of Nutrition 



263 



as implemented, generally involves intentional or de facto reduction in 

carbohydrate. Moreover, calorie restriction is too broad a term to provide 

much insight on mechanism, whereas we have substantial understanding 

of the biochemical effects of individual macronutrients, and especially the 

role of insulin. 


Research in carbohydrate restriction has probably underestimated the 

importance and value of detailed downstream stimulus—response coupling 

in cells, while cell biology has paid insufficient attention to the nature of 

upstream signaling—the effect of diet. It is not calories but carbohydrate 

that stimulates insulin release, and obesity is largely a response, not a stim¬ 

ulus. Ketone bodies, too, are vital to our evolving understanding. Originally 

considered primarily a marker for fat breakdown, ketone bodies are now 

understood to be a more global cell signal. The possibility of cancer treat¬ 

ment based on the theory that tumors might be more poorly adapted to 

use ketone bodies as an energy source because of their evolution during the 

life span of the individual is one specific approach tested in Eugene Fine’s 

small pilot study. 2 


More generally, the use of carbohydrate restriction as a cancer therapy 

might be the important battleground in the search for effective dietary 

treatment and prevention. The new scientific paradigm might also be 

better received in the area of oncology due to the failures of other methods 

to contain the disease. With regard to obesity, diabetes, and metabolic 

diseases, it may be necessary to clear out the backlog of biased, unscientific, 

and statistically flawed studies that have so far impeded progress. New 

standards will have to be implemented to improve the future literature. 

Scientific truth is its own justification, but in this case, relief of human 

suffering is what is truly important to society at large. Progress may be slow 

but the crisis is real and the stakes are high. 




ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 



T wo people, Monika Hendry and Paula Nedved, have made this 

book possible. Their continual encouragement, their probing 

questions, and their excellent proofreading abilities kept the book 

alive through periods during which there were many doubts and discour- 

agements. I am also grateful to Professor Wendy Pogozelski for her help 

with chapter 2 and for her enthusiasm about the final product. 


Insofar as this book is scientifically accurate it is due to my early influ¬ 

ences, particularly Thomas C. Detwiler, and later interactions with my 

colleagues, Doctors Eugene J. Fine, Frederick Sacks JefFS. Volek, Eric C. 

Westman, Jay Wortman, Steve Phinney, Ann Childers, and many others 

who have for so long served as loyal opposition to the medical and nutri¬ 

tional monarchy. 


Thanks also to members of the Nutrition and Metabolism Society and 

Facebook friends, as well as the greater social circle, Gary Taubes, Nina 

Teicholz, Carole Friend, Judy Barnes Baker, and especially the pseud¬ 

onymous Amanda B. Wreckondwith, who have provided continuous 

encouragement and endured a certain degree of curmudgeonly behavior. 

I am grateful to the State University of New York Downstate Medical 

Center, which has given me the freedom to work and think outside the box. 




NOTES 



Introduction 


1. W. Pogozelski, N. Arpaia, and S. Priore, “The Metabolic Effects of Low-Carbohydrate 

Diets and Incorporation into a Biochemistry Course,” Biochemistry and Molecular 

Biology Education 33 (2005): 91-100; R. D. Feinman and M. Makowske, “Metabolic 

Syndrome and Low-Carbohydrate Ketogenic Diets in the Medical School 

Biochemistry Curriculum/’ Metabolic Syndrome and Related Disorders 1 (2003): 

189-98; M. Makowske and R. D. Feinman, “Nutrition Education: A Questionnaire 

for Assessment and Teaching,” Nutrition Journal 4, no. 1 (2005): 2. 


2. F. B. Hu et al., “Dietary Fat Intake and the Risk of Coronary Heart Disease in Women,” 

New England Journal of Medicine 337, no. 21 (1997): 1491-99. 


3. M. Pollan, In Defense of Food: An Eaters Manifesto (New York: Penguin Press, 2008). 


4. M. U. Jakobsen et al., Major Types of Dietary Fat and Risk of Coronary Heart Disease: 

A Pooled Analysis of 11 Cohort Studies,” American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 89, 

no. 5 (2009): 1425—32; P. W. Siri-Tarino et al., “Meta Analysis of Prospective Cohort 

Studies Evaluating the Association of Saturated Fat with Cardiovascular Disease,” 

American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 91, no. 3 (2010): 535-46; P. W. Siri-Tarino et 

al., Saturated Fat, Carbohydrate, and Cardiovascular Disease,” American Journal of 

Clinical Nutrition 91, no. 3 (2010): 502-9. 


5. American Diabetes Association, “Nutrition Recommendations and Interventions for 

Diabetes 2008 J Diabetes Care 3 1 , Supplement 1 (2008): S61-78. 


6. A. Rabinovich, The Yom Kippur War: The Epic Encounter That Transformed the Middle East 

(New York: Schocken Books, 2004), 56. 


7. American Diabetes Association, “Nutrition Recommendations and Interventions for 

Diabetes 2013 J Diabetes Care 36, supplement 1 (2013): S12-32. 


8. N.Teicholz, The Big Fat Surprise: Why Butter, Meat & Cheese Belong in a Health Diet{ New 

York: Simon Sc Schuster, 2014). 


9. G. M. Reaven, “Role of Insulin Resistance in Human Disease,” Diabetes 37 (1988): 

1595-607. 


10. E. J. Fine, C. J. Segal-Isaacson, R. D. Feinman et al., “Targeting Insulin Inhibition as a 

Metabolic Therapy in Advanced Cancer: A Pilot Safety and Feasibility Dietary Trial in 

10 Patients,” Nutrition 23, no. 10 (2012): 1028-35. 


11. W. B. Kannel and T. Gordon, “Diet and Regulation of Serum Cholesterol, Section 25,” 

in The Framingham Study: An Epidemiological Investigation of Cardiovascular Disease 

(Washington, DC: National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, 1970). 



268 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Chapter 1: Handling the Crisis 


1. F. Hahn, M. R. Fades, and M. D. Eades, The Slow Burn Fitness Revolution: The Slow 

Motion Exercise That Will Change Your Body in 30 Minutes a Week (New York: Broadway 

Books, 2003). 


2. E. C. Westman, S. D. Phinney, and J. Volek, The New Atkins for a New You: The Ultimate 

Diet for Shedding Weight and Feeling Great Forever (New York: Simon & Schuster, 2010). 


3. M. R. Eades and M. D. Eades, Protein Power (New York: Bantam Books, 1996). 


4. J.S. Volek and S.D.Phinney, The Art and Science of Low-Carbohydrate Living (Charleston, 

SC: Beyond Obesity, 2011). 


5. R. D. Feinman, M. C. Vernon, and E. C. Westman, “Low Carbohydrate Diets in Family 

Practice: What Can We Learn from an Internet-Based Support Group,” Nutrition 

Journal 5 (2006): 26. 


6. S. Somers, Eat Greats Lose Weight (New York: Crown, 1997). 


7. R. C. Atkins, Dr Atkins New Diet Revolution (New York: Avon Books, 2002). 


8. R. C. Atkins, New Diet Revolution . 



Chapter 2: Whaddaya Know? 


1. American Diabetes Association, “Nutrition Recommendations and Interventions for 

Diabetes 2008.” 


2. American Diabetes Association, “Nutrition Recommendations and Interventions for 



Diabetes 2013.” 


3. American Diabetes Association, “Standards of Medical Care in Diabetes 2012, 

Diabetes Care 35, supplement 1 (2012): Sll-63. 


4. American Diabetes Association, “Standards of Medical Care in Diabetes 2010, 



Diabetes Care 33, supplement 1 (2010): SI 1—61. 


5. A. Keys, “Diet and Blood Cholesterol in Population Survey: Lessons from Analysis of 

the Data from a Major Survey in Israel,” American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 48, no. 5 



(1988): 1161-65. 


6. U. Raynskov, The Cholesterol Myths: Exposing the Fallacy That Cholesterol and Saturated 

Fat Cause Heart Disease (Washington, DC: NewTrends Publishing, 2000); G. Taubes, 

Good Calories, Bad Calories (New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 2007); A. Colpo, The Great 

Cholesterol Con (Morrisville, NC: Lulu Press, 2006). 


7. A. Keys, “Coronary Heart Disease in Seven Countries ,'”Annals of Internal Medicine 73, no. 

2 (1970): 356. 


8. N. Teicholz, The Big Fat Surprise ,; U. Raynskov, The Cholesterol Myths , G. Taubes, Good 

Calories, Bad Calories; G. Taubes, “The Soft Science ofDietary Tat,” Science 291 (2001): 



2536-45; U. Ravnskov et al., “LDL-C Does Not Cause Cardiovascular Disease: 

A Comprehensive Review of the Current Literature,” Expert Review of Clinical 

Pharmacology 11, no. 10: 959—970. 


9. K. Sarri and A. Kafatos,“The Seven Countries Study in Crete: Olive Oil, Mediterranean 

Diet or Fasting?,” Public Health Nutrition 8, no. 6 (2005): 666. 


10. N. Teicholz, The Big Fat Surprise . 


11. C. E. Forsythe et al., “Limited Effect ofDietary Saturated Fat on Plasma Saturated Fat 



the Cor 



jOW Carbohydrate Diet,” Lipids 45, no-10 (2010): 947-62: C. E. 



Notes 



269 



12 "i'^'“"‘ h 4?1 ""■ 5,2fJ i 30?-" 1 ' 10 ""’ 8 '“° J M '" b ' J “ 


13 fTh T” 4 ' ’ Un ' mJ °“"’ l °f c ‘"‘<°l°£y'H,M 


Chapter 3- The F' T DiS6aSe in Wome «-” 


s<W,i 992) *’“* J, " OT “(N™y„ rk .. Sin , on& 


4 rr WDietR£v ° lM ion. J ° Vanovi ch,1978). * 


' ^ — — «, Momingitar 


s' m 'Z oll T JnDefemeofFood - 



10. G. D. Foster et al. “Low-C k l"? ^ 2 ° 82 - 90 - 


11- P- L eren, “The Oslo Diet-Heart StudyW]^ 161 v" ^^ny-” 


935-42; P. Leren et a]., “The Oslo Study- CHDft "jfj? 0 "’” 42 > no. 5 (1970;- 


13 ^ ^ C0r0 ^ Disease,” CV* 28 


jo™: change 7 *« ^e 


ZZcJI 5 ’ n °' ! (2006); 39 ' 49; * * Howard ^ Me dkal 


£sk of Cardtovascuiar Disease: The Women’s Healrt T - Dieta V Pattem and 


657. ^ Cati0n TtU ’J^of iheAm ^MZZ Rand ° mized trolled 


D' ^° S t^ n0pal ^^ r ^^^^^ 1 ®' a ^’ni«^^Hedtli 1I I^ Kabetes Meflitus' 


” Ler i>asic NufnV;^«. at 



y «*« ivieaicint 


Chapter 4: Basic Nutrition- 7 U, 


1. M.Eades m P R j 771 -pj ‘ Mac ronutnents 


- anZ c,„„ &Gr ^, m 



270 



Nutrition in Crisis 



4. N.Teicholz, The Big Fat Surprise. rvF!ltan d Risk of Coronary Heart Disease: 


5. M-UJakobsen et X of Clinical Nutrition 89, 


A Pooled Analysis of 1 ° ^ ^ “Meta-analysis of Prospective Cohort 


no. 5 (2009): 1425-32; P. ■ in c atura t e d Fat with Cardiovascular Disease/ 


Studies Evaluating the Association o , 535-46; P. W. Siri-Tarino et 


Glinka!Nutrition 91, no. 3 (2010): 502-9. ^wCarbohydrateDietandaCalorie- 


6, ta Healthy 


Restricted Low Fa, Die, on 1617-23. 


Woma; Journal of CUnmlEnrktnm ogy a a Carbohydrate-fiee 


7. S. Borghjid and R. D. F=i»=W^ Mt6n mJ A .scent* 


Die,,' ^ Din A „ ociated with Sevete Gluco.e 


“Marked Hyperleptinemia After irlig ,™ no 4 (1998): 461-67; 


Intolerance in Mice,’ Eurof.JKJonrn.l ® ^ A ' Modcl fo, Studying 


M.S,Wi»adl.»dB,Ahten,-U.Htgh-F.,Dt d ft 2 D i, b e,.»- 


Mechanisms and Treatment of Imparted Glucose 


Trr' 


8 . S.Klein and R.R. Won, '-‘U y - ( 1992 ): E631-36. 


Fasting,” American.Journal of Physio ogy > • C A: Lange Medical 


9. H. A. Harper, Review of Physiological Chemistry, 8th ed. (Los Altos, 


Publications, 1961). h d (Los Altos, CA: Lange 


10. D. W. Martin et al., Harpers Rev t ev> ofBjochermstry, 20th 


Medical Publications, 1985). 


2 f'g'Y oung^'Claude ’Bernard and the Discovery of Glycogen: A Century of Retrospect," 


668-75. 


Chapter 6 : Sugar, Not Glucose-Sweetened, 


'■ L„g“n«»“ Vfeetal Adiposity »d Lipid, and 


Metabolism in the Liver 


" Co„tw“ »1 Lttpmeu, of Metabolic Syndrome,- — 


3. RDrebiman and E.J. Fine,"Fructose in Perspective,” Alwfriften andMotabolism 


‘ rpThubes^and C- K. Couzens,“Big Sugar’s Sweet Little Lies," Mother Jonos, 2012, 



Notes 



271 



Chapter 7: Saturated Fat: On Your Plate or in Your Blood? 


1. C E. Forsythe et al “Limited Effect of Dietary Saturated Fat on Plasma Saturated Fat 


Ee ^ ont “ t of a Low-Carbohydrate Diet "Lipids 45, no. 10 (2010): 947-62 


2. C. E Forsythe et al., “Comparison of Low Fat and Low Carbohydrate Diets on 


i Fatt ^ Aad Composition and Markers of Inflammation,” Lipids 41 

no. ! (20 8). 65-77; J. S. Volek et al„ “Dietary Carbohydrate Restricts Educes a 

mque Metabohc State Positively Affecting Artherogenic Dyslipidemia, Fatty Acid 

S y ndrome ’” jPro ^ in Lipid Research 47, no. 5 (2008): 


the Met h V S 6k f ’ ^ ° hydrate Restriction Has a More Favorable Impact on 

the Metabolic Syndrome Than a Low Fat Diet Lipids 44, no. 4 (2009): 297-309 


3. C. E. Forsythe et al., “Limited Effect of Dietary Saturated Fat ” 


4. S. K. Raate et al., “Total Fat Intake Modifies Plasma Fatty Acid Composition in Humans ” 


Journal of Nutrition 131, no. 2 (2001): 231-34. ’ 


Chapter 8: Hunger: What It Is, What to Do About It 


1. B- Behaviorism (New York: Knopf, distributed by Random House 1974) 


Fffi F ~. and E 'J' Fine > “Non-equilibrium Thermodynamics and Energy 

Efficiency in Weight Loss Diets,” Theoretical Biology and Medical Modelling 4 (2007): 27 


“ A CaW 18 3 “ orie ’• to 


TO* trBX%7w£ r ’ ° rd " " d “““ L “ ° f ^ Enrow (Nm 


2 C/^^/aT a1 '' ' Di ^ Comp ° s iti°n an d Energy Balance in Humans, Journal of 

Clinical Nutrition 67, no. 3 (1998): 551S-55S. J 


Chapter 10: Diabetes 


1. E_C. Westman and M. C. Vernon, “Has Carbohydrate-Restriction Been Forgotten as 

rea ment or Diabetes Mellitus? A Perspective on the ACCORD Study Design ” 

Nutrition and Metabolism 5 (2008): 10. 6 ’ 


2 ' Memr"T p W ' S ' Y r Cy ; J '-' “ d “■ H "” Phre?! ' »f Dinbere, 


“ W) 77-8 e 3 (1914 -‘ ,22 f p “^«- in «, 


3. D. J. Jenki™ et d "Effect of, Low-GIyconic Index or , High-Cereal Fiber Die, on 


4 f L C TT man 6 t *!i’ EffeCt ° f a L ° W “ Carbohydrate > Ketogenic Diet Versus 

a Low-Glycemic Index Diet on Glycemic Control in Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus ” 

Nutrition and Metabolism 5 (2008): 36. 


5. W. S. Yancy et al., “A Low-Carbohydrate, Ketogenic Diet to Treat Type 2 Diabetes ” 

Nutrition and Metabolism 2 (2005): 34. F J 


1' )l' St, 7 t f n, l a ^'’ 1 fffa ) D S5 0:Risk F ai;toreforI n eidenceandP,ogressionofR£dnoparhy 


m Type II Diabetes over 6 Years from Diagnosis,” Diabetologia 44, no. 2 (2001): 156-63 



272 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Chapter 11: Metabolic Syndrome: The Big Pitch 


1 . G. M. Reaven, “Role of Insulin Resistance in Human Disease,” Diabetes 37 (1988): 

1595-607; G. Reaven, “Syndrome X: 10 Years After,” Drugs 58, no. 1 (1999): 19-20. 


2. G. M. Reaven,” Role of Insulin Resistance”; G. M. Reaven, “Tie Metabolic Syndrome: 

Requiescat in Pace,” Clinical Chemistry 51, no. 6 (2005): 931—38. 


3 . J. S. Volek and R. D. Feinman, “Carbohydrate Restriction Improves the Features of 

Metabolic Syndrome. Metabolic Syndrome May Be Defined by the Response to 

Carbohydrate Restriction,” Nutrition and Metabolism 2 (2005): 31. 


Chapter 12: The Medical Literature: A Guide to Flawed Studies 


1 . M. C. Gannon and F. Q^Nuttall, “Control of Blood Glucose in Type 2 Diabetes without 

Weight Loss by Modification of Diet Composition "Nutrition and Metabolism 3 (2006): 16. 


2. R. D. Feinman and E. J. Fines, “Perspective on Fructose.” 


3 . G. A. Bray et al., “Effects of Dietary Protein Contention, Weight Gain, Energy 

Expenditure, and Body Composition During Overeating: A Randomized Controlled 

Trial," Journal of the American Medical Association 307, no. 1 (2012): 47-55. 


4. G. R. Norman and D. L. Steiner, PDQ Statistics, 3rd ed. (Hamilton, Ontario: B. C. 

Decker, 2003). 


5. D, Colquhoun, Lectures on Biostatistics (London: Oxford University Press, 1971), http:// 


www.dcscience.net/Lectures_on_biostatistics-ocr4.pdf. 


6 . H. Wainer, Medical Illuminations (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2014). 


7 . L. J. Appel et al., “Effects of Protein, Monounsaturated Fat, and Carbohydrate Intake 

on Blood Pressure and Serum Lipids: Results of the OmniHeart Randomized Trial, 

Journal of the American Medical Association 294, no. 19 (2005): 2455-64. 


Chapter 13: Observational Studies, Association, Causality 


1. S. Weinberg, Dreams of a Final Theory , 1st ed. (New York: Pantheon Books, 1992). 


2. S. Mukherjee, The Emperor of All Maladies: A Biography of Cancer (Waterville, ME: 

Thorndike Press, 2010). 


3. J. LeFanu, The Rise and Fall of Modern Medicine (New York: Carroll & Graf, 1999). 


4. J. LeFanu, The Rise and Fall of Modern Medicine . 


5. L. E. Cahill et al., “Prospective Study of Breakfast Eating and Incident Coronary Heart 

Disease in a Cohort of Male US Health Professionals,” Circulations 128, no. 4 (2013): 337-43. 


6 . S. Mukherjee, The Emperor of All Maladies , 246. 


Chapter 14: Red Meat and the New Puritans 


1. M. Pollan, In Defense of Food. 


2. R. Stein,“Daily RedMeat Raises Chances ofDying Early,March24,2009. 


3. R. Sinha et al., “Meat Intake and Mortality: A Prospective Study of Over Haifa Million 

People,” Archives of Internal Medicine 169, no. 6 (2009): 562-71. 


4. D. K. Layman et al., “A Reduced Ratio of Dietary Carbohydrate to Protein Improves 

Body Composition and Blood Lipid Profiles during Weight Loss in Adult Women,” 

Journal of Nutrition 133, no. 2 (2003): 411-17; D. K. Layman et al., “Dietary Protein 

and Exercise Have Additive Effects on Body Composition during Weight Loss in 



Notes 



273 



Adult Women, 1 "Journal of Nutrition 135, no. 8 (2005): 1903-10; M. P. Thorpe et al., “A 

in Protein, Dairy, and Calcium Attenuates Bone Loss over Twelve Months 

of Weight Loss and Maintenance Relative to a Conventional High-Carbohydrate 

Diet in Adults,’ "Journal of Nutrition 138, no, 6 (2008): 1096-100; P.J. Douglas et al, 

“Protein, Weight Management, and Satiety,” The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 

87, no. 5 (2008): 1558S-61S; P. J. Douglas et al., “Role of Dietary Protein in the 

Sarcopenia of Aging,” The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 87, no 5 (2008)' 

11562S-66S. 


5. R. Sinha et al., “Meat Intake and Mortality.” 


6. A. Pan et al., “Red Meat Consumption and Risk ofType 2 Diabetes: 3 Cohorts of US 

Adults and an Updated Meta-analysis,” The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 94 

no. 4 (2011): 1088-96. 


7. A. Pan et al., “Changes in Red Meat Consumption and Subsequent Risk ofType 2 

Diabetes Mellitus: Three Cohorts of US Men and Women,” Journal of the American 

Medical Association Internal Medicine (2013): 1-8. 


8. R. D. Feinman, “Red Meat and Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus J Journal of the American 

Medical Association Internal Medicine 174, no. 4 (2014): 646. 


Chapter 15: The Seventh Egg: When Studies Defy Common Sense 


1. S. Ebrahm et al., “Shaving, Coronary Heart Disease, and Stroke: The Caerphilly Study,” 

American Epidemiology 157, no. 3 (2003): 234-38. 


2. L. Djousse et al., Egg Consumption and Risk ofType 2 Diabetes in Men and Women,” 

Diabetes Care 32, no. 2 (2009): 295-300. 


Chapter 16: Intention-to-Treat: What It Is and Why You Should Care 


1. D. Newell, “Intention-to-Treat Analysis: Implications for Quantitative and Qualitative 

Research,” InternationalJournal ofEpidemiology 21, no. 5 (1992): 837-41. 


2. S. Hollis and F. Campbell, “What Is Meant by Intention to Treat Analysis? Survey of 

Published Randomized Controlled Trials,” AM/319, no. 7211 (1999): 670-74. 


3. G. D. Foster et al., “Weight and Metabolic Outcomes after 2 Years on a Low- 

Carbohydrate versus Low-Fat Diet: A Randomized Trial,” Annals of Internal 

Medicine 153, no. 3(2010): 147-57. 


4. Comparison of Current NSLP Elementary Meals vs. Proposed Elementary Meals,” 

Hunger-Free Kids Act of 2010, The White House, 2010, https://obamawhitehouse 

.archives.gov/sites/default/files/cnr_chart.pdf. 


Chapter 17: The Fiend That Lies Like Truth 


1. H. Wainer, Medical Illuminations (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2014). 


2. M. C. Gannon and F. Q^Nuttall, “Control of Blood Glucose in Type 2 Diabetes.” 


3. V. Hainer et al., “ATwin Study ofWeight Loss and Metabolic Efficiency,’ "International 

Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic Disorders 25, no. 4 (2001): 533-37. 


4. G. C. Smith and J. P. Pell, “Parachute Use to Prevent Death and Major Trauma Related to 

Gravitational Challenge: Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trails,” BMJ 

327, no. 7429 (2003): 1459-61. 



274 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Chapter 18: Nutrition in Crisis 


1. U. Ravnskov, The Cholesterol Myth: Exposing the Fallacy That Cholesterol and Saturated Fat 

Cause Heart Disease (Washington, DC: New Trends Publishing, 2000); U. Ravnskov, 

“Cholesterol: Friend or Foe?” 


2. R. D. Feinman and J. S. Volek, “Carbohydrate Restriction as the Default Treatment for 

Type 2 Diabetes and Metabolic Syndrome,” Scandinavian Cardiovascular Journal 42, 

no. 4 (2008): 256-63. 


3. A. Dahlqvists, LCHF-blogg (blog), 2014. 


4. H.Monery, “Dr. Gary Fettke’s‘Officially Cautioned,’” The Examiner,November 13,2016. 


5. W. Willett, personal communication to the author. 


Chapter 19: Cancer: A New Frontier for Low-Carbohydrate 


1. E. J. Fine, R. D. Feinman et al., “Targeting Insulin Inhibition as a Metabolic Therapy 

in Advanced Cancer: A Pilot Safety and Feasibility Dietary Trial in 10 Patients, 

Nutrition 28, no. 10 (2012): 1028—35. 


Chapter 20: The Future of Nutrition 


1. J. P. Ioannidis, “Meta-Research: The Art of Getting It Wrong,” Research Synthesis 

Methods 1 (2010): 169-84; J. P. Ioannidis, A. Tatsioni, and F. B. Karassa, “Who Is 

Afraid of Reviewers’Comments? Or, Why Anything Can Be Published and Anything 

Can Be Cited J European Journal of Clinical Investigation 40, no. 4 (2010): 285-87; 


R. Horton, “Offline: What Is Medicine’s 5 Stigma?,” The Lancet 385 (2015): 1380; 

M. Angell, Science on Trial (New York: W. W. Norton & Co., 1996); R. Feinman and 


S. Keough, “Ethics in Medical Research and the Low-Fat Diet-Heart Hypothesis,” 

Ethics in Biology, Engineering and Medicine 5, no. 2 (2014): 149—59. 


2. E.J. Fine, C.J. Segal-Isaacson, R. D. Feinman et al., “Targeting Insulin Inhibition as a 

Metabolic Therapy in Advanced Cancer: A Pilot Safety and Feasibility Dietary Trial in 

10 Patients ,”Nutrition 23, no. 10 (2012): 1028—35. 



INDEX 



Note: Page numbers followed by “f” refer to figures. Page numbers followed by “t” refer 

to tables. 



abdominal fat, 119 

absolute risk, 191,202,205,206,209, 

211-12 


absolute zero, 138,141 

Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 68 

acetoacetate, 101,104 

acetyl-coenzyme A, 76,97-100,101, 

103-4,120 

acetyldehyde, 99 

Adams, John, 15, 61 

Adams, Sam, 61 

addiction to sugar, 109 

adenosine diphosphate, 97,100 

adenosine triphosphate, 97,98,100,146 

adipocytes, 148 

Adler, Alfred, 31 


advanced glycation end-products (AGEs), 

94, 96 

aging, 94 


alcohol, 37, 73-74, 99,108-9 

Allen, Woody, 127,189,218 

Allison, David, 238 


American Diabetes Association, 11,13, 

44-45,57,151,161,176 

American Heart Association, 8,42,45,52, 

57,58,62 


on fructose, 107 

on saturated fat, 77 

on total dietary fat, 58,63 

American Medical Association, 62,177 

amino acids, 88, 89-90, 93,147,197-98 

ammonia, 90 



amylopectin, 75 

amylose, 75 


animal models, 82-83, 91-92,118,154 

anorexigenic hormones, 126,127 

antioxidant supplements, 224 

Appel, L.J., 180 

appetite, 126 


Art and Science of Low-Carbohydrate 

Livings 27 

ascorbic acid, 224 


association and causality, 185-96,206, 

235-36 


in carbohydrates and obesity, 40-42 

criteria on, 190-94,196 

in eggs and diabetes, 215-18,236 

in meats and disease, 198 

in smoking and lung disease, 185,190, 

192,195,196,199,236 

atherogenic dyslipidemia, 10,14,118,170 

atherosclerosis, 88 


athletes, carbohydrate loading in, 75 

Atkins, Robert, 32, 61-62,63,156 

Atkins diet, 25,27,29, 61-62 

breakfast in, 127 

cheese in, 32 


Foster study on, 64-66, 80-81,225-31 

generic status of, 13,29 

as high-calorie starvation diet, 85-86, 

101-2 


ketone bodies in, 29,102 

professional resistance to, 61, 62, 63, 65, 

69,156,168,228,256 



276 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Atkins diet ( continued ) 

sugar in, 107 


weight loss in, 13,23,65, 66, 67,153, 

225 


Atkins Nutritionals, 67-68 


atomic theory, 3 


Australia, 247—50 


average values in statistics, 237-39 


avocado oil, 55 


bacon, 1,168,210 

bacteria, glycolytic, 98-99 

beef tallow, 55,60 

bell-shaped curves, 237 

Bergman, Ingmar, 218 

Bernard, Claude, 91-94,153-54 

Bernstein, Carl, 199 

Bernstein, Richard, 163 

beta carotene, 224 

beta cells of pancreas, 42,159,216 

Big Fat Surprise (Teicholz), 13,55, 63, 78, 

250-51,262 


biochemistry, 3-4, 9,10,15 

black box approach to metabolism, 94—96, 

98 


Bloomberg, Michael, 105 

BMJ> 250 


body mass, 40,155-56 

Boxer, Barbara, 1 

brain, 43,76,101,102 

Bray, George, 177 

bread, 38-39,47,75,106 

breakfast, 1,2,124,127-28,191 

Brehm, Bonnie, 80, 81, 84 

Brillat-Savarin, J. A., 61 

butter, 38—39,47,77,118 


Cahill, George, 85,101-2 

“calorie is a calorie” idea, 23,32,38,84-85, 

126,133,138,177 

in cancer, 256 

and Hesss law, 144-46 



calories, 21,23, 37—38,39,40, 60 


in carbohydrates, 37,42,59, 84-85, 86, 

187,200 


consumption trends, 40,41f, 73 

in fat, 5,37-38,57,133 

in low-carbohydrate diets, 25, 32,33t, 


66 , 81, 84,135 


in low-fat diets, 25,30, 66, 81, 84,119, 

135 


reduction in intake of, 21,168,262-63 

and thermodynamics, 84—85,131-48 

calories in, calories out (CICO), 142 

calorimeters, 37-38 

Campbell, F., 223 

cancer, 172,191,253-59 


insulin in, 15,254—55,256-59 

ketone bodies in, 257,258f, 263 

low-carbohydrate diets in, 15,253-59, 

263 


of lung, 185,189-90,192,196 

and meat consumption, 201,204 

and smoking, 185,190,192,195,196, 

199 


Warburg effect in, 100,254 

candy, 30,105,165,186-87 

Cannon, Geoffrey, 54 

canola oil, 55—56 

caproic acid, 76 

carbohydrates, dietary, 73-76 


accuracy of dietary records on, 82, 84, 

135-36,227 


ADA on, 13,44-45,151,176 

calories in, 37,42,59,84-85,86,187,200 

and cardiovascular disease, 6—8,58-59, 

60,70,158-59 

catalytic effect of, 118 

consumption trends, 40,59,73,116 

conversion to fat, 88 

in DASH diet, 180-81 

in diabetes, 151—66. See also diabetes 

mellitus 


as fattening, 84—85, 86-89 



Index 



277 



feedback response to, 14 

and glucose levels, 2,21-22,42-43,59, 

151,155,164-65 

glycemic index of, 46-48,59, 83, 

156-58 


in high-carbohydrate diets. See high- 

carbohydrate diets 


insulin response to, 10,23,79, 80, 82, 

164,251,263 


intolerance of, 21-22,42-43,152,159, 

216,261 


and lipoprotein levels, 70 

in low-carbohydrate diets. See 

low-carbohydrate diets 

in low-fat diets, 118 

metabolism of, 86, 88,110-13 

nonsatiating effect of, 123 

in obesity, 40-42,59, 83,107,113,153 

recommended daily intake, 33t, 42, 

44-45,59 

refined, 83,262 


replacing fat with, 7, 8,58-59,60 

requirement for, 43,59,79,161 

and saturated fat in blood, 57, 88-89, 

121-22,170 

sugar as. See sugars 

and thermic effect of feeding, 141 

and triglyceride levels, 56-57,65,108, 

111,120,159,170,228-31 

carbon, 143-44 

carbon dioxide, 95, 99 

carbon monoxide, 143-44,145 

carbophores, 61 


cardiovascular disease, 10,191,193 

antioxidants in, 224 

and carbohydrates, 6-8,58-59, 60, 70, 

158-59 


cholesterol in, 4,5,15-16,48,56-58, 


63,70,159 

in diabetes, 159 


diet-heart hypothesis on. See diet-heart 

hypothesis 



and fats, 4-9,13,15-16,30,48, 51-54, 

62-64,69-70, 83,113,118,158-59, 

193,252,261 


Foster study on, 64-66,225-31 

Framingham Study on, 15-16, 62-63, 

69,193 


lipid markers of, 10,14,48-49 

and meat consumption, 201 

and metabolic syndrome, 14,167,170 

NHS study on, 6-8,58-59 

observational studies on, 188 

Seven Countries Study on, 53-55 

WHI study on, 64, 69,193 

carnivore diet, 79 

cauliflower, 29 


causality and association. See association 

and causality 


Center for Science in the Public Interest, 

55,250,251 


central nervous system, 101,102 

cheese, 32,54,106 

chemiosmotic theory, 100 

chemistry 


atomic theory in, 3 

of carbohydrates, 73-76 

energy in, 2, 96,97,98 

of lipids, 76-78 

chocolate, 106,123,161,165 

cholesterol, 13,48,56-59 


and cardiovascular disease, 4,5,15-16, 

48,56-58,63,70,159 

in low-carbohydrate diets, 65,70,120, 

159 


in low-fat diets, 56,57-58, 60,69-70, 


120 


Churchill, Winston, 262 

cigarette smoking. See smoking 

citric acid cycle, 90,99-100 

coconut oil, 52,118 

coenzymes, 97 


acetyl-CoA, 76, 97-100,101,103-4, 


120 



278 



Nutrition in Crisis 



Colbert, Stephen, 55 

Colquohon, David, 177-78 

Common Sense (Paine), 62,64,262 

compliance 


and intention-to-treat, 219-32 

in low-carbohydrate diets, 24,32, 

135—36,225,226-27,231,251 

self-reports on, 227 


in vitamin E supplementation, 224,230 

Consumer Reports, 52 

control loss in obesity, 25 

Cooksey, Steve, 68,248 

corn, 200 


corn syrup, high-fructose, 47,105,107, 

113,262 


coronary artery bypass surgery, 222-23 

coronary thrombosis, 190,196 

Couzens, Cristin Kearns, 115 

cravings, 30-31,32-33 

Crete, 53-55 

Crick, Francis, 181 


Dahlqvist, Annika, 246-47 

dairy fat, 121 

Dalton, John, 61 

DASH diet, 180-81 

degenerative disease, 94 

delayed-onset muscle soreness, 99 

de novo lipogenesis, 57, 78, 88,120—21 

depression, 110 _ 

desserts and sweets, 26,30-31,165 

candy, 30,105,165,186-87 

chocolate, 106,123,161,165 

cravings for, 30-31 


diabetes mellitus, 2,10,12—13,151—66 

ADA on diet in, 11,13,44—45,151, 

161,176 


carbohydrate intolerance in, 21-22, 

42-43,152,159,216,261 

compensatory feedback in, 14 

complications of, 11,153,159,164 

default diet in, 4,15,21-22,245,261 



drug therapy in, 11,22,28,44,45,151, 

158 


“eat to the meter” in, 22 

effectiveness of low-carbohydrate diet 

in, 28,42,59,106,113 

and eggs, 215-18,236 

epidemic of, 40—42,59,66,198,200 

fructose in, 113 


glucose blood levels in, 2,11,22,42-43, 

44,45,103-4 


glucose—insulin axis in, 79,153 

high-fat diet in, 42 

increase in incidence of, 116 

individualization of diet in, 13,20-21, 

176 


insulin production in, 21,42,103,152, 

154,159 


insulin resistance in, 14,21,42-43,103, 

152,154,159,216 

insulin therapy in, 152-53 

ketoacidosis in, 103-4 

latent autoimmune, 160-66 

long-term effects of low-carbohydrate 

diet in, 44—45 


and meat consumption, 213 

and metabolic syndrome, 14 

metabolism in, 4,43 

and obesity, 155—56,187-88 

principle of low-carbohydrate diet in, 33 

professional resistance to 

low-carbohydrate diet in, 10-11,13, 

15,22,44,248 

type 1. See type 1 diabetes 

type 2. See type 2 diabetes 

weight loss in, 22,153,155,156,176,235 

diet-heart hypothesis, 5-6,62-63 

in diabetes, 159 


lack of proof for, 9,13,58,60,63,70, 

83,235,261 

lipid levels in, 48,58 

low-fat diet in, 5—6, 9,13,48,261 

peer review of, 248 



Index 



279 



digestion, 46,74-75, 89-90,91,125 


disaccharides, 73 


Djousse, L., 215-18 


DNA, 181,254 


dog studies, 91-92,154 


Doll, Richard, 185,190,195 


Don’t Know Much About History , 61 


dose-response curve, 192-93 


drug therapy 


in coronary artery disease, 222-23 

in diabetes, 11,22,28,44,45,151,158 

in tuberculosis, 189-90 

dyslipidemia, atherogenic, 10,14,118,170 


Eades, Mary Dan, 32 

Eades,Mike, 32,187 

eating behavior, 123-30 

‘eat to the meter,”22,32 

eggs, 121,123,186,200 

and diabetes, 215-18,236 

Einstein, Albert, 9, 61,178,187 

elderly, diet of, 198,200,207 

electron transport chain, 99-100 

Elliott, Jennifer, 248,250 

Emperor of All Maladies, 185,188-89,194, 

195 

energy 


and calories, 21,37-38,39 

in carbohydrates, 28,40,45, 84 

from chemical reactions, 2, 96,97,98 

excess intake in diabetes, 44, 45 

in fasting, 86,101 

in fat, 37-38, 85,101 

from fatty acid oxidation, 87, 88 

from glucose, 87,94 

as goal of metabolism, 76,96-97 

from ketone bodies, 94,100 

and metabolic advantage, 132 

from protein, 90 

restriction in low-fat diet, 80, 81 

and thermodynamics, 85,131-48 

Enig, Mary, 78 



enthalpy, 143,144 

entropy, 138-41,143 

epidemiologic studies, 194-95,212 

accuracy of self-reports in, 227 

association and causality in, 187-89, 

190,192 


on fat in diet, 261-62 

on meat in diet, 197,198 

equilibrium constant, 96 

equilibrium thermodynamics, 146-48 

ester bonds, 49 

ethanol, 37, 73-74,99,108-9 

euricic acid, 56 


evidence-based medicine, 194,234-35, 

242,248,249 

evolution of diet, 77, 111 

exercise, 22,26,75,129 

extensive variables, 39,47,60 


Facebook, 175-76 

fasting, 85-86, 87-88, 94 


glucose levels in, 85, 86, 87, 88,152, 

159 


intermittent, 20 

protein in, 88, 94 

fat, body, 49, 79,88,119,131 

accumulation of, 148 

conversion of protein to, 90 

and eating behavior, 126-27 

in fasting, 85,86, 87, 88 

and glucose, 98,101,120,154 

oxidation of, 80, 84, 85, 86t 

role in metabolism, 86 

fat, dietary, 45-59,73,261-62 


accuracy of dietary records on, 82, 84, 

135-36,227 

animal models of, 82 

association and causality in, 188 

calories in, 5,37-38,57,133 

and cardiovascular disease, 4-9,13, 

15-16,30,48,51-54, 62-64,69-70, 

83,113,118,158-59,193,252,261 



280 



Nutrition in Crisis 



fat, dietary ( continued ) 

composition of, 55-56 

consumption trends, 40,41f, 73 

in diabetes, 40-42,43 

diet-heart hypothesis on, 5-6,9,13,48, 

62-63,83,252,261 

energy density of, 37-38 

epidemiologic studies on, 261-62 

and fat in blood, 48,117,119-20 

Foster study on, 64—66,68, 80-81,82, 

225-31 


Framingham Study on, 15-16,29, 

62-63,69,193 


good and bad, 76-78,226,261 

guidelines on, 42 


in high-fat diets. See high-fat diets 

in low-carbohydrate diet, 25,29—30,33, 

117,118,119-20,170,252 

in low-fat diets. See low-fat diets 

NHS on, 6-8,58-59 

in obesity, 5, 38,40-42, 83 


replacing carbohydrates with, 108,116 

saturated. See saturated fats 

storage of, 79,80, 84 

and thermic effect of feeding, 141 

unsaturated. See unsaturated fats 

WHI study on, 64, 69 

Fat Head documentary, 62,199 

fattening. See weight gain 

fatty acids, 48,49-56, 76-78 

in diabetic ketoacidosis, 103-4 

in fasting, 85, 86t, 87,88 

free, 49, 85, 86t, 103,104 

ketone bodies derived from, 101 

medium-chain, 52 

nonesterified, 49,51,103 

oxidation of, 87, 88,100,103,121 

plasma levels of, 121—22 

role in metabolism, 86 

saturated. See saturated fats 

structure of, 49,50f, 51,52, 53 

synthesis of, 57,88,120-21 



trans- and cis- configurations of, 51f, 

52-53,77,78 


unsaturated. See unsaturated fats 

Fay, Allen, 128-29 

feedback mechanisms, 13-14 

Feinman, Jeffrey, 84 

fermentation, 98—99 

Fettke, Gary, 247—48 

fiber, dietary, 156-57,179 

Fine, Eugene J., 15,102,110,188,255, 

257,259,263 

Finland, 53 

fish, 121 


flour, enriched, 107 

food pyramid, 1 

foods 


glycemic index of, 46-48,59, 83, 

156-58 


glycemic load of, 47,59 

oxidation of, 95, 98 

smell and taste of, 128 

Forsythe, Cassandra, 117,121,122 

Foster, Gary, 64—66, 68, 80—81, 82,225-31 

Framingham Study, 15-16,29, 62-63,69, 

193 


free fatty acids, 49,85, 86t, 103,104 

French Revolution, 91 

Freud, S.,20 

fructokinase, 112 

fructophobia, 30,107-9 

fructose, 44,47-48, 57,73,105-16 

chemistry of, 74 

and fructophobia, 30,107—9 

in high-fructose corn syrup, 47,105, 

107,113,262 

medical literature on, 177 

fruits, 28,32,64,187,200,208,226 

future of nutrition, 261-63 


Gannon, M. C., 155,156,176,235 

Gates, Bill, 237 

Gaussian distribution, 237 



Index 



281 



gender differences, 33t, 40,41f, 171,201-2, 

207 


genetic factors, 90,181 


in cancer, 253,254,256,257 

in cardiovascular disease, 9,58, 70 

Gibbs free energy, 142-43 

glucagon, 85, 87,103 

glucokinase, 113 

glucometers, 22 


gluconeogenesis, 14,43, 87, 88,133,146 

Bernard studies of, 91-94,154 

in diabetes, 103,155 

fructose in, 111 


liver in, 14, 76, 87, 92,93,103,153-54, 

155 


in low-carbohydrate diet, 76 

protein in, 76, 87, 88, 89, 90, 92, 93, 98, 

101,154 


glucose, 73,76,97-98,120,254 

and acetyl-Co A, 76,98,120 

amount in diet, 113-15 

Bernard studies on, 91-94,153-54 

brain requirements for, 43, 76,101,102 

in cancer, 254,257 

chemistry of, 74, 74f 

compared to fructose, 110-13 

conversion of fructose to, 105,108, 111, 

112f 


conversion to fructose, 110 

daily requirements for, 102 

and fatty acid synthesis, 120 

glycogen storage of, 14,43, 75-76, 87 

in high-fructose corn syrup, 105,113 

oxidation of, 98,100,143,146 

in sucrose, 105 


synthesis of. See gluconeogenesis 

glucose blood levels, 76,93—94 


carbohydrates affecting, 2,21-22, 

42-43,59,151,155,164-65 

constant, as goal of metabolism, 87,96 

in diabetic ketoacidosis, 103-4 

drugs lowering, 11,22,44,45 



in fasting, 85, 86, 87, 88,152,159 

feedback system in, 14 

and gluconeogenesis, 154 

and glycemic index of foods, 46—48, 

156-57 


in hyperglycemia, 21,43,76,103,154, 

164-65 


in hypoglycemia, 22, 76,164-65 

and insulin, 3,11, 61, 79-80,103-4, 

151,154-55,164 


in low-carbohydrate diets, 85-86,164—65 

in metabolic syndrome, 14,167,168t, 

170,171 


glucose-insulin axis, 40, 79-80,153,155, 

159,262 


in metabolic syndrome, 167,169 

glucose tolerance test, oral, 152,159 

glucose transporter type 4 (GLUT4), 103, 

154,257 

glycation, 76, 94 


glycemic index, 46-48,59, 83,156-58 

glycemic load, 47,59 

glycerol, 48,49,50f, 87 

glycogen, 14,23,43,73,75-76 

Bernard studies on, 91-94,153 

in fasting, 87, 88 


in low-carbohydrate diets, 23, 75-76 

in muscle, 75, 87 

structure of, 89 

synthesis of, 93,108, 111 

glycolysis, 98-99,100,254 

Good Calories, Bad Calories , 62, 63, 78 

Gordon, Tavia, 63 

grains, 22,30, 61, 64 

grapefruit diet, 118 


Handler, Philip, 62 

Harpers Illustrated Biochemistry, 86 

.Harvard Health Blog, 210-13 

Harvard School of Public Health, 27,45, 

197,215,242,251 

epidemiology studies, 212,227 



282 



Nutrition in Crisis 



hazard ratio, 203-6,217 

Head-and-Shoulders effect, 172 

healthful diets, 180-81,234 

Healthy Hunger-Free Kids Act, 226 

hemoglobin Ale, 152,157,159,161 

Hesss law, 144-46 

Hewat, Claire, 248 


high-carbohydrate diets, 33t, 79,89,106, 

108,116 

ADA on, 151 

animal models of, 82-83 

with high-fat diet, 30 

insulin in, 79,80, 82 

in metabolic syndrome, 168,169 

triglyceride levels in, 108, 111 

high-density lipoprotein, 48,56-58,60, 

65,70 


in atherogenic dyslipidemia, 14,118,170 

in high-carbohydrate diet, 111 

in low-carbohydrate diet, 159,170 

in metabolic syndrome, 135,168t, 170 

high-fat diets, 79, 82,117 

in diabetes, 42 


with high-carbohydrate diet, 30 

saturated fats in, 121-22 

high-fructose corn syrup, 47,105,107,113, 

262 


Hill, Bradford, 185,188-94,195,196,199, 

206 


Hill, James, 227 

Hite, Adele, 86 

Hollis, S.,223 

homeostasis, 132,135 

hormones 


and eating behavior, 126,127 

insulin as. See insulin 

Hu, Frank, 6,251 

Huckabee, Mike, 168 

Hufhngton Post, 13 

Hume, David, 189 

hunger, 21,26,123-30 

in low-fat diets, 24,30 



hydrogen, 95, 96 

hydrogenated oils, 52, 77—78 

p-hydroxybutyrate, 86t, 101,104,25 8f 

hyperglycemia, 21,43, 76,103,154, 


164-65 


hypertension in metabolic syndrome, 10, 


14,167,168t, 170 

hypertriglyceridemia, 57 

hypoglycemia, 22, 76,164—65 

hypothalamus, 126 


hypothesis in observational studies, 186, 

187,236,240 


hypothetico-deductive systems, 185 


ice cream, 31, 47-48 

imagery techniques, 129—30 

inborn errors of metabolism, 75, 90 

In Defense of Food (Pollan), 197 

Institute for Justice, 248 

insulin, 3—4,5,11,14,102-4 


anabolic effects of, 61,79,102,127,251 

in cancer, 15,254-55,256—59 

dosage administered, 151,153 

in fasting, 85, 86,87 

and fat storage, 80,84 

in feedback system, 14 

and fructose, 113 

and fruit intake, 32 

functions of, 79, 80,102 

and glucose blood levels, 3,11, 61, 

79-80,103-4,151,154-55,164 

in glucose—insulin axis, 40,79-80,153, 

155,159,167,169,262 

in high-carbohydrate diet, 79, 80, 82 

and ketoacidosis, 103-4 

in latent autoimmune diabetes, 160,161 

and lipase activity, 147f, 148 

in lipogenesis, 120 


in low-carbohydrate diets, 5,23, 85-86, 

121,151,251,252 

in metabolic syndrome, 10,14,167, 

168,169 



Index 



283 



production in type 1 diabetes, 21,42, 

103,152,154,159 

resistance in type 2 diabetes, 14,21, 

42-43,103,152,154,159,216 

response to dietary carbohydrates, 10, 

23, 79, 80,82,164,251,263 

and sucrose-containing foods, 11,44,151 

insulin pumps, 164 

insulin resistance 


in metabolic syndrome, 167,168,169 

in type 2 diabetes, 14,21,42-43,103, 

152,154,159,216 

intensive variables, 39,47, 60 

intention-to-treat, 66-67,182,219-32 

irritable bowel syndrome, 172 


Jacobson, Michael, 55,250 

Japanese food, 107 

Jenkins, David, 156-57 

junk food, 110,128 


Kaplan, Bob, 227 

ketoacidosis, diabetic, 103-4 

ketogenic diets, 23,27,28-29,34 

in athletes, 75 

in cancer, 15,255,257 

coconut oil in, 52 

in diabetes, 158 

popularity of, 4 


very low-carbohydrate, 33t, 118-20, 

135-36,171f 


ketone bodies, 43,52, 90,100-102,263 

in blood (ketosis), 28-29,34,101,103, 

257 


in cancer, 257,258f, 263 


in diabetic ketoacidosis, 103-4 


as energy source, 94,100 


in fasting, 85, 86, 94 


in ketogenic diets. See ketogenic diets 


synthesis and utilization of, 94, 


100-101,103 

in urine (ketonuria), 28 



ketonemia, 104 

ketonuria, 28 


ketosis, 28-29, 34,101,103,257 


Keys, Ancel, 53-54 


Klein, S., 85 


Krai, John, 126 


Krebs, Hans, 99 


Krebs cycle, 99-100 


lactic acid, 98, 99 

Lanaspa, Miguel, 110 

lard, 51,55,77 


latent autoimmune diabetes, 160-66 


Lavoisier, Antoine, 91 


Lederhandler, Izja, 195 


LeFanu, James, 189 


Levy, Barbara, 1 


lifestyle recommendations 


in cardiovascular disease, 225,227 

in type 2 diabetes, 152 

lipase, 147f, 148 

lipids, 4,45-59,76-78 


and atherogenic dyslipidemia, 10,14, 

118,170 


chemistry of, 76-78 

cholesterol. See cholesterol 

diet-heart hypothesis on, 48,58 

fatty acids. See fatty acids 

metabolism of, 83—84 

triglycerides. See triglyceride levels 

lipogenesis, 57(78, 88,120-21 

lipolysis, 87,103,104,148,251 

lipophobia, 8, 53, 63, 64, 67,107,246, 

262 


meat in, 198 


unintended consequences of, 109 

lipoproteins, 48,56-59,70 


in atherogenic dyslipidemia, 14,57, 60, 

118,170 


high-density. See high-density 

lipoprotein 


low-density. See low-density lipoprotein 



284 



Nutrition in Crisis 



liver, 75,87,112f, 113 


acetyl-coenzyme A in, 103,104 

in cholesterol synthesis, 5 

in glucose synthesis, 14, 76, 87, 92, 93, 

103,153-54,155 

glycogen in, 14,75, 87,92,153 

Lorenzo's Oil\ 56 

Los Angeles Times , 227 

Low-Carber Forums, 29 

low-carbohydrate diets, 3-4, 8,23-25, 

151-72 


accuracy of dietary records in, 82, 84, 

135-36,227 

ADA on, 13,44-45 

AHA on, 57 

AMA on, 62 

in athletes, 75 


Atkins diet as. See Atkins diet 

calories in, 25,32, 33t, 66, 81, 84,135 

in cancer, 15,253-59,263 

in carbohydrate intolerance, 21-22,261 

and cardiovascular disease, 70,158—59 

cheese in, 32 


cholesterol levels in, 65,70,120,159 

compared to low-fat diets, 65-70, 

80-82,118-20,134f, 135-36,169-70, 

171f, 225-31,246 


compared to low-glycemic index diet, 


156-58 


compliance with, 24,32,135-36,225, 

226-27,231,251 

cravings in, 30-31, 32-33 

desserts and sweets in, 26,30-31 

in diabetes, 151—66. See also diabetes 

mellitus 


fat in, 25,29-30,33,117,118,119-20, 

170,252 


first revolution in, 61-70 

Foster study on, 64-66,68, 80-81, 

225-31 

fruits in, 32 

future of, 261-63 



glucose blood levels in, 85—86,164—65 

glycemic index in, 46-48 

glycogen in, 23, 75-76 

as high-calorie starvation diet, 85-86, 

101-2 


insulin in, 5,23, 85-86,121,151,251, 

252 


ketogenic. See ketogenic diets 

medical education on, 3-4, 69,169, 

247-48 


medical literature on, 177 

metabolic advantage in, 131,132-33 

in metabolic syndrome, 15,21,28, 70, 

79,106,113,167-72,261 

metabolism in, 3-4,23,28,75-76,131, 

132-33,252 


nuts and nut butters in, 32 

in obesity, 113 

overeating in, 32-33 

in overweight, 79,106 

principles of, 27-33 

protein in, 89-90,101 

resistance to, 10-11,13,15,22,44, 

61-63,65,68-69,156,168,176,228, 

245-51,256,261 


satiety in, 23,24—25,32-33,66,127,251 

second revolution in, 12-13,243-63 

sugar in, 30—31,105—6,113 

techniques for staying on, 26 

triglyceride levels in, 57, 65,120,159, 

170,228-31 

types of, 33-34 

vegetables in, 28-29 

in weight loss. See weight loss programs 

Westman study of, 157-58 

low-density lipoprotein, 48,56-58, 65,70 

in atherogenic dyslipidemia, 57,60,118 

particle size of, 57-58,167 

pattern B, 58,60,118 

low-fat diets, 4—5, 8,24,29—30, 62 

accuracy of dietary records in, 82, 84, 

135-36,227 



Index 



285 



calories in, 25,30,66, 81, 84,119,135 

and cholesterol levels, 56,57-58, 60, 

69-70,120 


compared to low-carbohydrate diet, 

65-70, 80-82,118-20,134f, 135-36, 

169-70,171f, 225-31,246 

diet-heart hypothesis on, 5-6,13,48, 

261 


failures of, 262 

hunger in, 24,30 

in metabolic syndrome, 168,169 

nutritionist support of, 68-69 

in obesity, 5, 38 

political policies on, 109 

recommendations on, 76-77 

research findings on, 15-16,29-30,54, 

63-70,80-82,135-36 

saturated fat and fatty acids in, 29-30, 

117,119-20,121 

triglyceride levels in, 228-31 

low-glycemic index diet, 156-58 

lung cancer, 185,189-90,192,196 

Lustig, Robert, 105,107,108 


malonyl-coenzyme A, 120-21 

margarine, 77,109 

Maxwell, James Clerk, 140 

Maxwells demon, 138-39,140 

Mayes, Peter, 88 

McDonald s, 55 

McGovern, George, 62 

meat consumption, 186,197-213 

in carnivore diet, 79 

epidemiologic studies on, 59 

in low-carbohydrate diet, 28,33 

and satiety, 123 

warnings on, 27, 90 

medical education, 3-5,245,247-48 

on diabetes and nutrition, 162 

on fat in diet, 79 


on low-carbohydrate diets, 3-4,69,169, 

247-48 



quiz on nutritional concepts in, 35-60 

on scientific papers, 180 

Medical Illuminations ,, 233 

medical literature, 175-242. See also 

research studies 


Mediterranean diets, 27, 53-55 

menopause, 64 

Men's Healthy 123 


meta-analysis, 81,192,236-40,261 

metabolic advantage, 131,132-33,141,239 

metabolic syndrome, 10,14-15,167-72 

insulin in, 10,14,167,168,169 

low-carbohydrate diet in, 15,21,28, 70, 

79,106,113,157-58,167-72,261 

Volek study of, 117-22,135,169-70, 

171f 


metabolism, 3-5,10,15, 91-104 

acetyl-CoA in, 76,97-100,101 

in cancer, 254-59 

of carbohydrates, 86, 88,110-13 

in diabetes, 4,43 

in fasting, 85-86, 87-88,94 

fuels in, 76, 97-98,120 

gluconeogenesis in. See gluconeogenesis 

goals of, 76, 87, 88,96-97 

homeostasis in, 132,135 

inborn errors of, 75, 90 

insulin in, 79,80,102-4 

lipogenesis in, 57, 78, 88,120-21 

in liver, 112f, 113 


in low-carbohydrate diets, 3-4,23,28, 

75-76,131,132-33,252 


of sugar, 110-13 

mice studies, 82-83 

milk, 1 


Milk, Harvey, 109 

minerals, 73 

Mitchell, Peter, 100 

moderate-carbohydrate diet, 33t 

monosaccharides, 73 


monounsaturated fat and fatty acids, 6-7, 


49,50f, 51-52,55,121 



286 



Nutrition in Crisis 



mortality rate 


in coronary artery disease, 222-23 

in intention-to-treat analysis, 224 

in meat consumption, 197-213 

Mother Jones, 115 

Mukherjee, Siddhartha, 185 

muscles, 96, 99 


and glycogen, 75, 87 

and ketone bodies, 101 

and protein, 89,90 

starvation affecting, 20,101 

Mussolini, Benito, 31 


Nabel, Elizabeth, 64,226 

National Academy of Sciences, 62 

National Health and Nutrition 

Examination Survey, 40,42 

National Institutes of Health, 48,63,64, 

200 - 201,212 

Nature , 110 

Naughton,Tom, 62 

New Atkins for a New You, 27 

Newell, David, 222-23 

New EnglandJournal of Medicine, 27 

Newton, Isaac, 221 

New York Times Magazine, 62 

nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide, 97-98, 

99 


Njurunda Medical Center, 247 

Noakes, Tim, 249-50 

nonesterified fatty acids, 49,51,103 

Nordmann, Alain, 81 

Norman, G. R., 178 

nose cancer, 192,199 

number needed to treat (NNT), 206 

Nurses’Health Study, 6-8,58-59 

nut butters, 32 

Nutrition, 255 


nutrition, basic rules of, 19-22,176 

nutritional epidemiology, 187,188-89 

Nutrition and Metabolism, 110,169—70,220 

Nutrition in Crisis, 246 



nuts, 32,121 


Nuttall, E Qy 155,156,176,235 


oatmeal, 2,30 

Obama, Michelle, 226 

obesity, 10,25,116 

and cancer, 254,256 

carbohydrates in, 40-42,59,83,107, 

113,153 


and diabetes, 155-56,187-88 

and diet sodas, 192 

epidemic of, 39-42, 59,66,73,107, 

198,200 


and fat in diet, 5, 38,40-42, 83 

and metabolic syndrome, 14,168t 

sugar in, 105,186-87 

vagotomy in, 126 


observational studies, 185-96,235-36 

criteria on, 190-94,196 

hypothesis in, 186,187,236,240 

on meat consumption, 200 

odds ratio, 203-6,207 

oils, 48,49,52,55,77-78 

oleic acid, 51, 52,55,60,76,121 

olive oil, 51,52,53,55,60,121 

orange juice, 106 

orexigenic hormones, 126 

Oslo Diet Heart Study, 29,69 

overeating, 32-33 

overweight, 79,106,124,153 


and metabolic syndrome, 10,14,135, 

167,170 


oxidation, 95-100 

and calories, 38,60 

of carbon, 143-44 

of fat, 80,84, 85, 86t 

of fatty acids, 87, 88,100,103,121 

of glucose, 98,100,143,146 

of protein, 145—46 


oxidation-reduction reactions, 95-96,97-98 



Paine, Thomas, 62 




Index 



287 



paleo diets, 27-28 

palmitic acid, 57, 76, 88,120,121 

Pan, A, 210,211,212,213 

pancreas, 21,42,159,216 

parachute use, 241-42 

Pasteur, Louis, 94 

PDQ Statistics , 178 

peer review, 27,175,213,248,261 

PellJ.P, 241-42 

Phinney, Steve, 75 

phosphorylation, oxidative, 98 

physics, 38,96 

Planck, Max, 246 

Pogozelski, Wendy, 160-66 

Poisson distribution, 237 

politics, 10,13,15,16,109 

Pollan, Michael, 8,63,197 

polyol reaction, 110 

polysaccharides, 46, 73, 74 

polyunsaturated fat and fatty acids, 6-7,49, 

51,121 


Pop-Tarts, 107 

portal vein, 93 

postabsorptive state, 87 

postmenopausal women, 64 

postprandial state, 87 

potatoes, 28,47 

Prigogine, Ilya, 138 

Principia, 221 

probability, 139,140 

prospective experiments, 188 

protein, 73, 89-90,197-98 

calories in, 37 


consumption trends, 73,198,200 

in fasting, 88, 94,101 

in glucose synthesis, 76, 87, 88, 89, 90, 

92,93,98,101,154 

in low-carbohydrate diet, 25, 33,101, 

118,135 


in low-fat diet, 118,135 

oxidation of, 145-46 

satiating effects of, 123 



and thermic effect of feeding, 141 

Protein Power, 21 , 32,187 

proteinuria, 90 


Psychopathology of Everyday Life, 20 

Public Health Nutrition, 54 

Puritans, 197 

pyruvic acid, 98-99,254 


quantum mechanics, 131 


quiz on nutritional concepts, 35-60 


Raatz, S. K., 122 

Rabinovitch, Abraham, 11-12 

randomized controlled trials, 158,189,193, 

240-42,246 

rapeseed, 55-56 

Ravnskov, Uffe, 246 

Reaven, Gerald, 14,167 

red meat, 27,59,186,197-213 

redox reactions, 95-96, 97-98 

reinforcement of behavior, 128-30 

relative risk, 190-91,196,198,201-6,209, 

211 


research studies, 175—242 


animal models in, 82-83, 91-92,118, 

154 


association and causality in. See 

association and causality 

on cancer, 255,256-59,263 

common sense in, 200,234,235,242 

comparison of low-fat and 

low-carbohydrate diets in, 65-70, 

80-82,135-36,169-70,225-31 

consistency of results in, 192 

on diet-heart hypothesis, 62-63,248, 

261 


on egg consumption, 215-18 

epidemiologic. See epidemiologic 

studies 


of Foster, 64-66, 68,80-81, 82,225-31 

Framingham Study, 15-16,29,62-63, 

69,193 



288 



Nutrition in Crisis 



research studies ( continued ) 

on healthful diets, 180-81 

inclusion criteria in, 223-25 

intention-to-treat in, 66-67,182, 

219-32 


interpretation of, 12,180 

levels of evidence in, 182 

on low-fat diets, 15—16,29-30,54, 

63-70,80-82,135-36 

on meat consumption, 197-213 

meta-analysis of, 81,192,236—40,261 

misrepresentation in, 181 

number of subjects in, 236,240 

Nurses’Health Study, 6—8,58-59 

observational. See observational studies 

peer review of, 27,175,213,248,261 

prospective experiments in, 188 

randomized controlled trials in, 158, 

189,193,240-42,246 

self-reported diet records in, 82, 84, 

135-36,202,216,227 

self-serving descriptions in, 234—35 

Seven Countries Study, 53-55 

specificity in, 192,199 

statistics in. See statistics 

visual representations on, 233-34 

of Volek, 75, 89,117-22,135-36,170 

of Womens Health Initiative, 29,64, 

69,193,226 


Reverse Mussolini Fallacy, 31 

Review of Physiological Chemistry, 86 

rice, 27,59,177,179 


Rise and Fall of Modern Medicine, 78,189 

risk, 190-91 


absolute, 191,202,205,206,209, 

211-12 


from eggs, 215-18 

from meat, 198,200—213 

and probability, 208 

relative, 190-91,196,198,201-6,209, 

211 


Robinson, Jackie, 3,11 



rodent studies, 82-83,118 


sandwich, invention of, 189 

satiety, 123-30 


in low-carbohydrate diets, 23,24—25, 

32-33,66,127,251 

saturated fats, 48,60,77-78,117-22 

and cardiovascular disease, 6—8,29—30, 

51,53-54,58-59,64,77,118 

compared to unsaturated fat, 49 

consumption trends, 40, 41f 

dietary carbohydrates affecting blood 

levels of, 57, 88-89,121-22,170 

in low-carbohydrate diets, 117,119-20, 

170 


in low-fat diets, 29-30,117,119—20, 


121 


research findings on, 59,78, 89,117-22, 

170,246,250-51,261 

structure and form of, 50f, 51,52,53,78 

synthesis of, 57,78, 88,120-21 

Sboros, Marika, 248 

school lunch programs, 226 

science, 195,196 


association and causality in, 187—88, 


189 


philosophy of, 185,189,195 

self-deception about evidence, 11-12 

self-reported diet history, 227 

on egg consumption, 216 

on fat and carbohydrate intake, 82,84, 

135-36,227 


on meat consumption, 202 

Semmelweis, Ignaz, 11 

Seven Countries Study, 53—55 

j Seventh Seal, 218 


Shakespeare, William, 27,197,233 

Sharon, Ariel, 12 

significance, statistical, 198,240 

Singer, Isaac Bashevis, 209 

Sinha, Rashmi, 199,200,206 

Siri-Tarino, R W., 8 



Index 



289 



60 Minutes, 107 

Skinner, B. F. } 125 

smell of food, 128 

Smith, G. C., 241-42 

smoking, 2,115 


and cancer, 185,190,192,195,196,199 

and coronary thrombosis, 190,196 

hazard ratio in, 206 

snacking, 124 

sodas, 1,30,31,186,192 

Somers, Suzanne, 32 

Sommerfeld, Arnold, 131,136 

sorbitol, 110 

South Africa, 249-50 

sperm cells, 110 

standard deviation, 237-38 

starch, 30,44,46,73,108,112,113 

in bread, 75 

chemistry of, 74f, 75 

in diabetes, 2,106,153 

overconsumption of, 107 

in weight-loss diets, 1 

starvation, 20, 85-86, 88,101-2 

statistics, 177-79,194 


absolute risk in, 191,202,205,206,209, 

211-12 


averages in, 237-39 

and common sense, 200 

on egg consumption, 215-18 

hazard ratio in, 203-6,217 

intensity measures in, 39 

in intention-to-treat, 219-32 

interpretation of, 12 

on meat consumption, 200-213 

number needed to treat in, 206 

in nutritional epidemiology, 194 

odds ratio in, 203-6,207 

probability in, 208 

relative risk in, 190-91,196,198, 

201-6,209,211 

significance in, 198,240 

standard deviation in, 237-38 



standard error of the mean in, 80 

two-tailed distribution in, 207 

visual presentation of, 233-34 

stearic acid, 55,120,121 . 

stearoyl-CoA desaturase-1,121 

Stein, Rob, 199 

Stewart, Potter, 185 

Streiner, D. L., 178 

streptomycin in tuberculosis, 189-90 

sucrose, 47,73, 74f, 105,110 


in diabetic meal plan, 11,44,151 

sugar alcohols, 67-68,73 

sugar industry, 115 

sugars, 1,73-75,91-94,105-16 

dangers attributed to, 262 

in desserts and sweets, 30-31 

in diabetes, 44,153 

as easy target, 246 

fructose. See fructose 

in fruits, 32 

glucose. See glucose 

in low-carbohydrate diets, 30—31, 

105-6,113 

and obesity, 186-87 

refined, 83 


research findings on, 177,195 

in sodas, 1,30 


sucrose, 11,44,47, 73,105,110,151 

surgery, coronary artery, 222—23 

Sweden, 246-47 

sweeteners, 31,113-15 

sweets. See desserts and sweets 


table sugar, 105 

taste, 128 


Taubes, Gary, 62,63, 64,115,255 

Teicholz, Nina, 55,63,250-51,262 

temperature regulation, 126 

thermic effect of feeding, 133,141 

thermodynamics, 84—85,127,131-48,187 

thermogenesis, 141 

thrombosis, coronary, 190,196 



290 



Nutrition in Crisis 



tobacco industry, 115 

trans-fats, 5 If, 52,55,56,63,77,78,109 

triacylglycerols, 49,50f, 118,120,122,148 

tricarboxylic acid cycle, 99-100,108 

triglyceride levels, 49,56-58 


in high-carbohydrate diet, 108, 111 

in low-carbohydrate diet, 57,65,120, 

159,170,228-31 

in low-fat diet, 228—31 

in metabolic syndrome, 135,168t, 170 

triose phosphates, 111, 112 

tuberculosis, 189-90 

Twain, Mark, 203 

Twinkles, 107,109 

twins, weight loss in, 238-39,239f 

two-tailed distribution, 207 

type 1 diabetes, 21 


insulin in, 21,42,103,152,154,159 

ketoacidosis in, 103-4 

latent autoimmune, 160-66 

type 2 diabetes, 21,248 

complications in, 159 

early symptoms in, 153 

and eggs, 215-18 

glucose blood levels in, 42-43 

insulin in, 14,21,42-43,103,152-53, 

154,159,216 


lifestyle recommendations in, 152 

low-glycemic diet in, 156-57 

and meat consumption, 213 


United Kingdom Prevention of Diabetes 

Study, 159 


unsaturated fats, 48,51-52,55,60,77-78, 

121-22 


and cardiovascular disease, 6-7,58-59 

monounsaturated, 6-7,49,51—52,55, 

121 


polyunsaturated, 6—7,49,51,121 

structure and form of, 52,53,77 

urea, 90 


USDA, 1,20,42,69,107,226,250,251 



vagotomy, 125-26 

vagus nerve, 125-26 

vegetable oils, 6, 52,55, 77 

vegetables, 28-29,64,187,198,200,208, 

226 


vegetarian diets, 90,197—98,204 

Vegetarian Times , 197 


very low-carbohydrate ketogenic diets, 33t, 

118-20,135-36,171f 

vitamins, 2, 73 


and intention-to-treat, 224,230 

Volek, Jeff, 24,42, 75, 89,117-22,135-36, 

169-70,251 

Vowell, Sara, 197 


Wainer, Howard, 233 

Warburg effect, 100,254 

Washington Post, 199,200,201 

water, 95 


Watergate scandal, 199 

Watson, James, 181 

weight gain 


in animals, 61 


carbohydrates in, 84—89,107 

and eating behavior, 127 

energy density of foods in, 38-39 

sugar in, 105 


weight loss programs, 1,3,20-21 


Atkins diet in, 13,23,65,66,67,153,225 

average weight loss in, 238 

behavioral reinforcement in, 128-30 

breakfast in, 127-28 

calorie reduction in, 21, 38 

cheese in, 32 


compliance with, 24,32,251 

cravings in, 32-33 


in diabetes, 22,153,155,156,176,235 


effectiveness of low-carbohydrate diets 


in, 4,13,67,81,245,251 

exercise in, 22,129 

fasting in, 20, 88 

Forsythe study of, 121 



Index 



291 



Foster study of, 65-66, 80-81,225,226, 

227 


gender differences in, 40,171 

graded response in, 28,34 

intention-to-treat analysis of, 220-21 

ketone bodies in, 28,34 

low-fat diet compared to 

low-carbohydrate diet in, 65-66, 

80-81,118-19,135,225 

metabolic advantage in, 131,239 

in metabolic syndrome, 14 

personal benefits of low-carbohydrate 

diet in, 251-52 

professional resistance to 

low-carbohydrate diet in, 62 

satiety in, 23,24-25,32-33,66,127,251 

scientific literature on, 61-62 

sugar in, 105 

Swedish endorsement of 

low-carbohydrate diet in, 247 

thermodynamics in, 136 

twin studies of, 238-39,239f 

Volek study of, 24,118-19,135,251 

Weinberg, Steven, 185 



Western diets, 40 


Western Electric Study, 69 


Westman, Eric, 46,151,153,156,157-58 


wheat, 200 


White, Dan, 109 


Willett, Walter, 6,32,249 


Wills, Gary, 61 


Wilmot,John, 124 


Wolfe, R. R., 85 


women 


carbohydrate and fat intake in, 33t, 41f, 

64 


glucose-insulin axis in, 40 

meat consumption in, 201-2,207 

weight loss in, 171 


Womens Health Initiative, 29, 64, 69,193, 

226 


Woodward, Bob, 199 

Wordy Shipmates, 197 


Yancy, William, 158 

yogurt, 99 

YouTube, 107 

yo-yo dieting, 24 



ABOUT THE AUTHOR 



RICHARD DAVID FEINMAN is a professor 

of cell biology at the State University of 

New York Downstate Medical Center in 

Brooklyn, New York, where he has been 

a pioneer in incorporating nutrition into 

the biochemistry curriculum. A graduate 

of the University of Rochester and the 

University of Oregon (PhD), Dr. Feinman 

has published numerous scientific and 

popular papers. Dr. Feinman is the founder 

and former coeditor-in-chief (2004-2009) of the journal Nutrition and 

Metabolism . His current research interest is in the application of ketogenic 

diets to cancer. 




The Nutrition and Metabolism Society 




“[A] Fascinating book ... by one of the original 

low-carb researchers whose grounding in the 

field goes back decades.” 


—Nina Teicholz , author of The Big Fat Surprise 


“A must-read for anyone with a serious interest 

in health and nutrition.” 


-Michael R. Eades.MD, 


author of Protein Power 


“Every scientific discipline needs a Dr. Feinman. He 

is just as skeptical about what he believes as what 

he disbelieves. His writing bears eloquent testi¬ 

mony to why he is such a scientific treasure.” 


-Timothy Noakes , MD, emeritus professor, 


University of Cape Town; founder, 

The Noakes Foundation 


“This cant-put-it-down title is the closest thing 

to a complete, popular analysis of the biochem¬ 

istry of human nutrition that you will find and a 

superb lesson in how scientific studies have been 

manipulated to prove fiction.” 


-Richard K. Bernstein , MD, author of 


Dr. Bernsteins Diabetes Solution 


“This book tells an informative and entertaining 

story about what went wrong in the nutrition 

establishment and advocates for a rational solu¬ 

tion to the problem.” 


—Dominic D’Agostino, PhD, leading 


scientist on ketogenic metabolic therapies 



A lmost every day it seems a 

new study is published 

that shows you are at risk 

for diabetes, cardiovascular disease, 

or death due to something youVe 

just eaten for lunch. Many of us no 

longer know what to eat or who to 

believe. In Nutrition in Crisis ., distin¬ 

guished biochemist Dr. Richard 

Feinman cuts through the noise, 

explaining the intricacies of nutrition 

and human metabolism in accessible 

terms and providing a commonsense 

approach for making intelligent 

nutritional choices. 


Nutrition in Crisis offers an 

unsparing critique of the nutritional 

establishment, which continues to 

demonize fat and ignore the benefits 

of low-carbohydrate and ketogenic 

diets—all despite decades of evidence 

to the contrary. Entertaining, infor¬ 

mative, and irreverent, Dr. Feinman 

paints a broad picture of the nutri¬ 

tion world that shows the beauty 

of the underlying biochemistry, the 

embarrassing failures of the medical 

establishment, and what’s wrong 

with the constant reports that the 

foods we’ve been eating for centu¬ 

ries represent a threat rather than a 

source of pleasure. 



Chelsea Green Publishing 

85 North Main Street, Suite 120 

White River Junction, VT 05001 

(802)295-6300 

www.chelseagreen.com 


Cover design by Melissa Jacobson 



$24.95 USD 





worth it" 


pRmej on recvo-Ed paper 


PRINTED IN CANADA 



ISBN TPfl-l-bOBSfl-an-S