Folk science describes ways of understanding and predicting the natural and social world, without the use of rigorous methodologies (see scientific method). One could label all understanding of nature predating the Greeks as "folk science." Folk science is often accepted as "common wisdom" in a given culture and gets passed on from generation to generation. In psychology and anthropology, scholars use the term folk science to characterize systems of knowledge relying either on intuition or on empiricism in its crudest form. Folk science is often thought of as laypeople’s mechanistic understandings of the natural and the artifactual worlds.
All of us, from the most sophisticated adults to the youngest children, often engage in what is commonly called “folk science,” that is, certain ways of understanding the natural and artificial world that arise more informally and not as direct reflections of formal instruction in scientific principles (Carey, 1988). Without explicit instruction in areas of science, people seem to develop domain-specific ways of thinking about relatively bounded sets of phenomena such as the behavior of solid objects, living kinds, and the minds of others (Carey, 1985; Carey & Spelke, 1996; Slaughter & Gopnik, 1996).
"Folk science gets it wrong because we evolved in a radically different environment."- Michael Shermer
"We find that nearly all Americans—irrespective of socio-economic status, political orientation, and educational background—endorse at least some aspects of Medical Folk Wisdom (MFW)" - Motta and Callaghan (2020)
Folk Science originates from people trying to explain the world around them through a relatively macroscopic lens. The large gap between the folk science perspective and a modern scientific perspective of the world lies in the difference in technology between now and prior time periods in human history. For example, we now have the ability to perceive the world in an extremely acute way due to the use of high-definition microscopes and rigorous testing procedures.
Folk Science is one of the earliest forms of explanation for the workings of the observable world. Prior to the onset of logic and reasoning initiated by the ancient Greeks, everyday people relied on "common wisdom" to help explain the world around them.
Feed a cold, starve a fever
Chewing gum stays in your stomach forever
Swallowing pits/seeds will make a tree grow in your stomach
Tarot card reading
What goes up must come down.
A dropped object falls straight down.
A solid object cannot pass through another solid object.
The Earth is flat.
Celestial bodies revolve around Earth.
The heavier ball falls faster than the lighter one.
Don't swim after eating
Hot tubs prevent pregnancy
Deciding who subscribes to folk science is not easy. Historically, everyone is subject to believe in folk science at some level. The extent to which each person subscribes to folk science is determined by many factors including their religious beliefs, level of education, political views, and cultural domination.
Despite having highly impoverished understandings of the world at the mechanistic level, children and adults alike have strong interests in mechanistic explanations. These interests in mechanisms may support the development of folk-scientific understandings by enabling even the very young to build a sense of causal patterns that exist far above the level of mechanisms. That sense of causal patterns then works in combination with strategies for identifying and evaluating both experts and their explanations, enabling laypeople of all ages to supplement their highly incomplete knowledge by accessing and relying on the divisions of cognitive labor that exist in all cultures (Keil, 2012).
There are many scenarios of how folk science is passed on and delivered. These scenarios depend on the setting of practice and the audience. Folk science ideas can be passed on from the source to the audience through the idea of creating a role model. For example, in educational environments or community service fields, educators or community leaders may represent a source of transmission of folk science ideas in the form of setting a role model to their followers. In these scenarios, there is usually an absence of the scientific method. This absence of science-based thinking and lack of critical thinking facilitates the transmission of these folk science ideas.
Another field of transmission is the family structure and household. Parents represent the figures that their children follow. In some families which impose a highly controlled environment and limited exposure to external experiences, children inherit their parents' ideas without subjecting them to critical reasoning. These ideas, by default, are seen as the "ideas" and the ideal ones. Currently, social medial trends and influencers greatly contribute to the transmission of ideas, practices, and even delusions. The viral spread of trends on social media rarely gives enough time for questioning their content or for mindful examination of evidence.
Believing in folk science can be harmless, however, sometimes It can have negative consequences and impacts on one life or society as a whole. In a study done by Matthew Motta and Timothy Callaghan on The pervasiveness and policy consequences of medical folk wisdom in the U.S., the researchers found that despite controlling certain factors such as economic status, political orientation, and educational background, Americans who believed in folk wisdom are less likely to value medical expertise. People who believe in folk science, are less likely to support evidence-based health policies which in return can lead to a lower level of public support of such policies. This can be similar to folk science beliefs in other fields as well, which can push those who believe in folk science to stay distant from evidence-based science which in return can significantly impact the publics' view on science.
According to Scientific American, the reason folk science so often gets it wrong is that we evolved in an environment radically different from the one in which we now live. Our senses are geared for perceiving objects of middling size--between, say, ants and mountains--not bacteria, molecules and atoms on one end of the scale and stars and galaxies on the other end. We live a scant three score and 10 years, far too short a time to witness evolution, continental drift or long-term environmental changes. Taking this into consideration, one way we can avoid falling for folk science-based misconceptions is by creating secondary science education lessons that address these concerns. Namely, students should engage in activities where they familiarize themselves with the scale of the universe in terms of time and size (example activity). Additionally, by ensuring that students have mechanistic explanations for scientific phenomena such as continental drift, they will be less likely to contribute it to folk science beliefs. That can be achieved by having students explore maps featuring the distribution of ancient fossils, rocks, and mountain ranges.
Although children may occasionally ask “why” or “how” questions as ways of merely engaging adults, they often expect mechanism-rich responses from them. So why not give it to them? For example, if a child asks why the sky is blue and is told that all skies are blue (or worse, is given a folk science explanation), she will be more likely to then ask more “why” questions than she would if she had been given a mechanistic answer about air particles absorbing certain colors of light (such mechanistic answers can also invite further “why” questions, but they do so to a lesser extent). Also, in providing the child with a mechanistic explanation, we avoid having them develop folk-scientific understandings.
One of the big challenges is the ability of individuals to critically evaluate online information. Students are typically directed to utilize a checklist approach as they evaluate online information. This means that students carry a list of questions to consider as they examine digital content online.