The difficulty of L2 speech learning
One of the most arduous tasks a language learner faces is mastering the pronunciation of the foreign language. Second language speech research has proposed several reasons to why accurate pronunciation of a foreign language is so difficult (e.g. Flege, Munro, & MacKay, 1995). In this section, I will take a look at the acquisition of L2 pronunciation from the point of view of the connectionist approach on language learning and the Noticing Hypothesis (Schmidt, 1995).
Neural and motoric re-structuring
From the neurological point of view, the adult L2 learner’s brain has already been committed to the configurations of the L1 phonological system. Accurate perception (and consequently, production) thus requires the overriding of the pre-existing L1 neural connections (Ellis, 2002).
L2 pronunciation is also partly dependent on the speaker’s motoric skills: sometimes we are able to perceive individual phones and rhythmic patterns accurately but when we try to produce them, they do not come out quite as we hear them in our head. Accurate L2 production entails the reconfiguration of articulatory movements, which since early infancy have been wired for the pronunciation of the L1. Additionally, in order for the speech to be fluent, the application of the articulatory movements has to be fast and automatic.
In other words, developing accurate and fluent L2 pronunciation entails the overriding of the perceptual and articulatory settings of the L1.
The difficulty of noticing phonological features
From the cognitive point of view, the L2 learner has to notice the target L2 pronunciation features in order to learn them (the Noticing Hypothesis, Schmidt, 1995) (more about noticing in here). Noticing leads the learner to become gradually aware of the differences between the L1 and the L2, a process which leads to subtle changes in the interphonology.
Let us take as an example the acquisition of L2 vowels. Initially, the learners assimilate given vowels to the L1 as they have not yet noticed differences between the quantity and quality of the vowels in the two languages. However, after exposure to more and more exemplars and most likely following some kind of communication failure (sixty/sixteen, pool/pull), the learners may notice some distinctive features which lead to the approximation to more target-like pronunciation.
There are no guarantees that the learner will notice all the relevant phonological aspects, such as quality, quantity and tenseness in the previous example of vowel acquisition. The final state of the representation is not necessarily complete and accurate. In fact, most adult L2 learners do not consciously notice all the features of the L2 phonology and/or perceive their own faulty input, as attested in numerous perception and production studies in which L2 learners have been found to identify, discriminate, categorize and produce L2 speech differently from native speakers. We could ask what makes the noticing of phonological aspects so difficult.
On the one hand, the very inherent nature of pronunciation poses challenges for its noticing. Language users are more concerned with meaning of what is said than with the form of how it is said, and only attend to form when attentional resources have not been depleted by attending to the meaning (VanPatten, 1996). This implies that only more proficient language learners, whose attentional resources are not needed anymore on deciphering the meaning, are able to focus on the formal characteristics of the L2. Once the language learner has reached this advanced stage, attention on the form of L2 speech will still complete for attention on the form of L2 grammar (e.g. morphemes) and the lexicon (e.g. orthography) as well as other aspects. Additionally this view suggests that once attention is freed for pronunciation, comprehensibility (being understood) is preferred over accuracy (speaking without a foreign accent). Taken this into account, it is not surprising that learners may never come to notice L2 phonological features.
The above-mentioned issues make the noticing of L2 phonological features challenging in normal daily communication situations. Noticing could be enhanced, to some extent, if those language learners who learn the L2 in a classroom setting would receive explicit instruction about the pronunciation of the L2 in the same manner they receive instruction about the grammar and lexicon. Nevertheless, it is widely acknowledged that this is not the case. In a regular language class, pronunciation is most often addressed systematically only in relation to item-learning and when communication is endangered (‘Did you say feel or fill?).
In this section, some of the reasons to why language learners rarely achieve native-like pronunciation of a foreign language were discussed. The second language learners need to restructure the neurological and motoric pathways employed since early infancy in L1 speech to accommodate to the L2. Additionally, they need to have enough attentional resources and disposition to notice the relevant aspects of L2 phonology. As phonological aspects are not as salient as semantic or grammatical aspects, for example, and students rarely receive explicit pronunciation instruction, noticing L2 phonology is a complicated matter.
Adapted from Kivistö-de Souza (2015)
Ellis, N. C. (2002). Reflections on frequency effects in language processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 297-339.
Flege, J., Munro, M., MacKay, I. R. A (1995). Factors affecting strength of perceived foreign accent in a second language. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 3125-3134.
Kivistö-de Souza, H. (2015). Phonological awareness and pronunciation in a second language (unpublished doctoral dissertation). Universitat de Barcelona, Spain.
Schmidt, R. (1995). Consciousness and foreign language learning: a tutorial on the role of attention and awareness in learning. In Schmidt, R. (Ed.), Attention and awareness (pp. 1-63). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai`i, National Foreign Language Resource Center.
VanPatten, B. (1996). Input processing and grammar instruction in second language acquisition. Norwood: Ablex Publishing