Now, we will explore how inner speech develops throughout childhood and into adulthood using a stage theory based on Vygotsky’s research (for a reminder on what that is, see the first section, What is Inner Speech?). I will discuss the purpose of each stage in its development, approximately when each stage occurs, and some examples at each stage.
After an introduction of the stages of inner speech development, I will introduce some other interesting quirks of inner speech development in the Further Exploration section.
Below is an infographic summarizing the stages (content adapted from a paper by Vissers et al, 2020); I will elaborate more in the text below.
STAGES: A Visual/Textual Exploration
STAGE 1
When children learn their first language, they start speaking out loud to communicate with other children and with adults. This is the first stage of inner speech development, which is also known as the external dialogue stage.
For example, Lenny, a preschool student, may say “Stop!” when poked too many times by his friend Bob.
STAGE 2
As children practice their speaking and listening skills more and more, they transition into stage 2 of the developmental model: private speech. This stage, on average, begins around the age of 3-4. In this second stage, children will start speaking to themselves out loud, engaging in self-regulation, or guidance they give themselves, sounding much like a scolding or supportive adult as they comment on their current activity.
For example, Lenny may build a sandcastle in the sandbox and comment out loud “Good job!” to himself for his great building work.
STAGE 3
Once children begin thinking silently to themselves about tasks such as math, reading, or writing, they have entered stage 3 of the model: expanded inner speech. Expanded inner speech develops around the age 6-7, it focuses on activities happening in the present or in the future, and it has been compared to a top-down process (another top-down process is perception, when our brain organizes data to create a conclusion) since brain scans of expanded inner speech use similar regions of the brain.
For example, Lenny, now in first grade, struggles through a math problem: One plus one equals what again? he thinks. I’ll hold up one finger for each one… oh right, two.
STAGE 4
The final stage of inner thought, condensed inner speech, is spontaneous and unconscious. It is ungrammatical, fast, and reflects the thinker’s current experience. This process resembles bottom-up processing in brain scans.
For our final stage, Lenny has grown up and is a financial analyst at his friend Bob’s company. As he thinks about preparing for his big presentation to a client, an excerpt from his inner speech looks something like this: forgotkeyswalletohno gotta calculate profit margins slidesnotpretty howfix? This example also demonstrates that as children grow into adults, their inner speech often becomes more and more abbreviated.
A note about the last two stages: Vygotsky had these two stages combined, but recent research by the guy from the YouTube video in the last section, Fernyhough, has suggested that we should have them as two separate stages.
Stage 1: Lenny's annoying friend
Stage 2: Lenny's private success
Stage 3: Lenny's newfound literacy
Stage 4: Lenny's final form (his thought is abbreviated now)
Some researchers have tested the effects of having an imaginary companion on inner speech.
One significant study tested five year old children, holding factors like family wealth constant, by interviewing them about how they talk to themselves (and their imaginary friend if they had one).
The researchers found that overall, children who had an imaginary friend tended to engage in inner speech (silent self-talk) more than those who did not. These children also had more fantasy-related vocabulary in their self-talk.
The results of this study support Vygotsky’s idea that social interaction plays an important role in inner speech development – previous studies have shown that the imaginary friends play a similar social role to real friends (and do not usually compensate for real friends), so greater access to social interaction due to having an imaginary friend could be the reason the children with one tended to engage in more inner speech.
This particular question provides many opportunities for deeper investigation – if you are interested, check out Davis, P. E., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2013) in the references at the bottom of this page, or the SciShow video on the right of this text ->
As we have explored, having an imaginary friend tends to elicit more (and richer) inner speech in children
More on imaginary friends from SciShow:
An example of inner speech at stage 2 development from Winsler's 2003 study on inner speech and classroom behavior.
Given what we have learned so far about the role of inner speech (hint: see the first bullet of STAGE 2 on the infographic), it should not be surprising that researchers have investigated if inner speech might play a role in classroom behavior and social skills in children.
One study approached this question by testing children from ages 3 to 5. They found that children on the younger side of the spectrum engaged in private speech (STAGE 2) more often, and that children on the older side transitioned towards internal speech (STAGE 3), supporting the general age ranges from the infographic at the beginning.
They also found that children who internalized this self-talk more had better social skills as reported by their parents, and better classroom behavior as reported by their teachers.
So far, we have learned that inner speech develops in stages, beginning from speaking out loud and ending in abbreviated, fast silent thought.
Further, both of the phenomena that we just discussed (inner speech with imaginary friends, social skills and classroom behavior with inner speech) suggest a two-way relationship between inner speech and thought quality.
While more research is certainly needed before we form any conclusions, the current evidence points to frequent inner speech being a good thing/sign for a developing child.
In the next section, we will take a look at pathologies that can interfere with inner speech.
References (check these out for further reading!)
Berk, L. E., & Garvin, R. A. (1984). Development of private speech among low-income Appalachian children. Developmental Psychology, 20(2), 271–286. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.20.2.271
Davis, P. E., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2013). Individual differences in children's private speech: the role of imaginary companions. Journal of experimental child psychology, 116(3), 561–571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.010
Vissers CTWM, Tomas E and Law J (2020). The Emergence of Inner Speech and Its Measurement in Atypically Developing Children. Front. Psychol. 11:279. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00279
Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/1986). Thought and Language, trans. by Alex Kozulin. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Winsler, A., De Leon, J. R., Wallace, B. A., Carlton, M. P., & Willson-Quayle, A. (2003). Private speech in preschool children: Developmental stability and change, across-task consistency, and relations with classroom behaviour. Journal of Child Language, 30(3), 583-608.