Nurjihan Balqis binti Nur Khan (214547)
Language acquisition is the process whereby children acquire their first languages. All humans (without exceptional physical or mental disabilities) have an innate capacity to acquire language. Acquisition materialises passively and unconsciously through implicit learning. Children do not need direct instruction to learn their first languages, but instead, they seem to “pick up” language in the same way they learn to roll over, crawl, and walk.
AL BARAA
Al Baraa is mainly exposed to Arabic in verbal and written form. Though his father knows and talks a little bit of French, Arabic is the primary language spoken at home. Having his father a manager of a book publishing company, he is exposed to a panoply of Arabic books, which he sometimes tries to open to see the pictures or illustrations. As the son of the researcher’s friend, whom he privately teaches Business English course at the comfort of his own home, Al Baraa is always around whenever the researcher teaches his Dad, playing around with his toys, disturbing his Dad, and talking to him whenever he pleases. According to his father, he can speak in basic Arabic at his age, though he speaks with grammatical errors.
He can fairly comprehend 2-3 connected sentences in a conversation. He cannot respond meaningfully to phrases beyond three sentences. Thus, his discourse competence is still very low. On sociolinguistic competence, it was observed that he is on an intermediate level. He knows how to adjust his language depending on the person he speaks with. He is more spontaneous when he speaks to his father and holds some reservations about the researcher as a stranger. He can also distinguish if his father is angry or joking, tired, or interested to listen to him. As a young child learning his first language, it is given that he has finite grammatical competence.
Consequently, he finds tribulation in effectively expressing himself in a communicative situation. However, it was observed that he could use verbal and non-verbal communicative strategies (strategic competence) to compensate for his communication complications. For example, when he wants something from his father but does not know its word, he can point to the object or demonstrate it through action. In some samples, he also uses simpler, related terms to lead the listener to arrive at the message he wants to express.
RELATION
Al Baraa’s first language acquisition is mainly innate or biological (Innateness Theory). His possession of a standard and functional human brain enables him to perceive or receive linguistic inputs and produce communicative outputs. His overt behaviours are embodied in several linguistic tasks that confirm Lenneberg’s nativist theory of first language acquisition of a child, Chomsky’s innateness hypothesis, and McNeill’s Language Acquisition Device (LAD) (Orillos, 1998). For example, possessing a human brain capable of processing inputs allows him to respond suitably to a given communicative situation. It would then be impossible for him to understand or utter words without a central processing unit – the brain, which contains specific parts with corresponding linguistic functions.
It was also observed that the subject learned some Arabic words and sentences (though sometimes faulty) without undergoing any formal instruction. Considering his very young age, the amount of his linguistic knowledge (vocabulary, grammar, phonology, syntax, etc.) can be surprising to someone. According to his father, he learned some words on his own. He also establishes his linguistic codes and systems. This could reflect the naturalness of language acquisition and learning. Language acquisition happens as a person grows or matures, hence, Chomsky’s innateness hypothesis (Orillos, 1998).
Aside from biological aspects playing significant roles in Al Baraa’s language acquisition, other factors are considered, which include neurological, psychomotor, cognitive, and affective considerations (Orillos, 1998). By the age of 3, it is already clear that language acquisition is controlled by his left hemisphere as manifested by his right-handedness. His inability to pronounce words with “r” sound is also explained by Krashen’s (in Orillos, 1998) theory that lateralization is only complete by the age of 5. It is only after five that a child masters the original articulation. Since Al Barraa is only 3, body parts and mechanisms responsible for producing the “r” sound are not yet fully developed (psychomotor considerations). Hence, this evidence proves the relation between the topic of first language acquisition and the real-world example provided.
BACKGROUND
The child that is being studied is named Al Baraa. He is a 3-year-old Muslim Lebanese born to a middle-class Arabic family in Saudi Arabia. He is the first child of a couple who is educated up to Bachelor’s degrees in Lebanon. He has a brother and a sister who are in their babyhood and babyhood stages. His father is headquartered in Lebanon, while his mother is simply a housewife. A physically, mentally, and socially healthy boy loves playing with toy cars, running inside the house, watching Youtube videos, and touching every strange thing he sees. Still, on his way to acquiring his first language, he always tries to question his mother or father the names of things, actions, or ideas as he observes inside and outside the house.
Language of any sort is exceedingly complex, yet it is utterly amazing how children at a very young age (0-5 years old) can acquire or master it on their very own. During their Linguistic Period (1-5 years old), children subconsciously accomplish complex tasks like linking sentences, asking questions, using appropriate pronouns, negating sentences, forming comparative clauses, and inflecting verbs and nouns (Fromkin, Rodman, and Hyams, 2010). However, sometimes, with some lapses or imperfections, they can orally answer questions and participate in conversations and argumentations. Some clever children can also tell whether an adult is lying or not.
This event makes anyone particularly keen on language learning wonder how young children learn such complex structures and functions of a language without going to school or experiencing any formal instruction. A number of linguists are attempting to explain such a humanly overwhelming phenomenon. For instance, Lenneberg, Chomsky, and McNeill (in Brown, 2007; Orillos, 1998; Erneling, 1993; Green and Peil, 2016; Islam, 2013; Heidar, 2012) share the common belief that language is a “specific” behaviour which is inborn in every individual. However, Bloomfield, Skinner, Fries, Hymes, Halliday, and Oller (in Brown, 2007; Orillos, 1998; Johnson, 2004; Ingram, 1989; Tahriri, 2013; Islam, 2013; Heidar, 2012; Ipek, 2009) believe that language is an acquired behaviour through conditioning and reinforcement, and that is learned through interaction in various situations and applications. Based on these opposing views, language acquisition is impacted and controlled by two influential factors – heredity (nature) and environment (nurture). The underlying query is which one is more influential over the other.
SOURCES
Meniado, J. C. (2016). First language acquisition: A case study of a three-year old Lebanese child.
Journal of Child Language Acquisition and Development, 4(3), 98-112.
Dani, E. P. (2014). THE DEVELOPMENT OF CHILD LANGUAGE ACQUISITION. Jurnal Mantik Penusa, 15(1). Retrieved from
http://www.e-jurnal.pelitanusantara.ac.id/index.php/mantik/article/view/251
INTERESTING ASPECTS
The fact that Al Baraa is right-handed is indeed interesting, as it indicates the dominant brain hemisphere being the left hemisphere, the region where the Language Acquisition Device (LAD) is harboured (Orillos, 1998). Even though the researcher could not physically inspect the Language Acquisition Device (LAD) or the left hemisphere of the child's brain, they could still determine if the parts were present and functioning faultlessly by observing the linguistic behaviours associated.
Al Baraa also speaks clearly except for the sound of "r" which is expected at his age. He does not have any speech imperfection; thus, his Broca's area is working well. He can also hear and recognize sounds, symbols, or letters and respond well when asked. He can understand words and utterances and intercommunicate meaningfully; thus, his Wernicke's area is also functioning well.
In general, Al Baraa has the biological (innate) facilities that allow him to comprehend and utilize a language. Without these, it would be unattainable for him to acquire, understand, and use language. No matter how healthy the environment is, if his body parts responsible for language learning are impaired, he cannot acquire any language. Thus, it has been proven that Chomsky's innateness theory (Orillos, 1998) has played a role in Al Baraa's language acquisition and development.
PERSONAL OPINION
In my own personal opinion, it is fascinating that every human is born with the capability to master a language and speak fluently in the language they were exposed to as a child. In addition, every language known is known for its respective complexity. Several theories sprung up with an attempt to uncover such a seemingly mysterious question but failed to clear up some doubts and spin-off questions completely. Though theories prescribe rationales to phenomena, they fail to attribute or recognize different personal circumstances and individual dissimilarities. Theories of Chomsky, Skinner, Hymes, etc., may be reasonable to be applied to some cases, but they may not be applicable to all individuals in different and unique circumstances.
However, in the case of Al Baraa, the acquisition of his first language is primarily intrinsic (Innateness Theory). His possession of a standard and active human brain helps him perceive and receive linguistic inputs and produce communicative outputs. These phenomena proved Lenneberg’s nativist theory, Chomsky’s innateness hypothesis, and McNeill’s Language Acquisition Device (LAD) (Orillos, 1998).
Nevertheless, Al Baraa evidently cannot fully understand lexical items and rules of morphology, syntax, semantics, and phonology (grammatical competence). Though he can construct sentences independently, his sentences are very basic. Sometimes the function words are omitted, thus, telegraphic sentences. When he uses analogy or substitution in sentences that he imitated from what he heard, he then makes his own. His choice of words might be unfitting or sometimes misplaced, thereby creating vagueness in the meaning.
CONCLUSION
First language acquisition is shaped and influenced mainly by biological and environmental factors. As manifested in the case of Al Baraa, he acquired his first language with the presence and aid of his Language Acquisition Device (LAD). Without such brain aptitude, it would have been inconceivable for him to acquire his first language – the Arabic language. There are several considerations in the natural acquisition of his speech – neurological, psychomotor, cognitive, and practical. The maturation of his brain parts is responsible for discharging linguistic functions, growing his muscles liable for producing speech sounds and eliminating emotions that delay the process of his language acquisition. These contributed to the natural evolution of his first language. Although his ability to acquire his first language might seem ingrained, the environment further contributed in his development and improvement in mastering his first language.
Bagaric, V., & Djigunovic, J. M. (2007). Defining communicative competence. Metodika, 8(1), pp. 94-103. Retrieved from
http://hrcak.srce.hr/file/42651.html
Dani, E. P. (2014). THE DEVELOPMENT OF CHILD LANGUAGE ACQUISITION. Jurnal Mantik Penusa, 15(1). Retrieved from
http://www.e-jurnal.pelitanusantara.ac.id/index.php/mantik/article/view/251
Heidar, D. M. (2012). First language acquisition: Psychological considerations and epistemology.
Theory and Practice in Language Studies, 2(2), pp. 411-416. doi:10.4304/tpls.2.2.411-416
Ingram, D. (1989). First Language Acquisition: Method, Description, and Explanation.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Meniado, J. C. (2016). First language acquisition: A case study of a three-year old Lebanese child.
Journal of Child Language Acquisition and Development, 4(3), 98-112.
Tahriri, A. (2013). Revisiting first language acquisition through empirical and rational perspectives. International Journal of Social Science &
Education, 3(3), pp. 677-682. Retrieved from
http://ijsse.com/sites/default/files/issues/2013/v3i3/Paper-14.pdf