Notwithstanding Kotov's forays into the political realm, his books were insightful and informative and were written in a congenial style. He often made his points by citing first-hand stories of incidents involving famous grandmasters, most of whom he knew personally. In addition, he did not hesitate to be self-deprecating if he could make a point more vividly. Think Like a Grandmaster illustrates several situations where his opponents got the better of him; in one case, his catastrophic blunder converted a certain win into an instant loss. Such entertaining and enlightening personal accounts helped to ensure that his books remained popular among chess players of widely varying nationalities and playing strengths.

His trilogy of books Think Like a Grandmaster, Play Like a Grandmaster, and Train Like a Grandmaster are his best known, with Think Like a Grandmaster, which was translated from the Russian by Bernard Cafferty and published by Batsford in 1971, being particularly famous. The book is not concerned with advising where pieces should be placed on the board, or tactical motifs, but rather with the method of thinking that should be employed during a game. Kotov's advice to identify candidate moves and methodically examine them to build up an "analysis tree" remains well known today.


Download Think Like A Grandmaster Pdf


Download 🔥 https://urllio.com/2y3Kmb 🔥



In Kotov's 1971 book Think Like a Grandmaster, he described a situation when a player thinks very hard for a long time in a complicated position but does not find a clear path, then, running low on time, quickly makes a poor move, often a blunder.[2]

My return to regular chess-playing has been accompanied by an interest in revisiting some of the chess books I read (ok, maybe it's more fair to say "looked at") when I was younger. Something I've been looking for in these classics besides positional insights and tactical advice are hints regarding the cognitive mechanisms that expert players bring to bear during play. Chess is a hobby for me, but cognitive science is my career - I'm a vision scientist by training, with research interests focused on visual recognition (faces and textures in particular). I'm also broadly interested in what are sometimes called visual routines, which refers to processes we use to understand spatial relationships visually: How do you work out a path through a maze, for example, or how do you work out which parts of a complicated diagram are connected? This latter topic has a lot of interesting overlap with chess and in the spirit of blending work with play as much as possible, I've been looking around for what connections others have already made between playing chess and the study of the mind. I've got a pretty good stack of books at the moment (I've got a TON to say about the intriguing work of Adriaan de Groot documented in his book Thought and Choice in Chess in particular), but one of the first I decided to tackle was Alexander Kotov's Think Like a Grandmaster. With a title like that, there's just gotta be some good cognition in there somewhere, right?

Lots of writers and chess players have tried to describe the psychological aspects of playing chess and I've been working on familiarizing myself with some of their ideas. One thing I'll say right away is that few of them have much to say with what I'd describe as the cognitive science of chess and are instead more oriented towards the psychology of chess. That distinction may not seem like a big one to you (or some of my colleagues in the academy), but to me it matters a great deal. The former discipline - cognitive science - is specifically focused on thinking about the mind in computational terms, with sub-domains concerned with perception, memory, attention, and decision-making and points of intersection with computer science, graphics, and philosophy. The latter is a broader field, which includes topics like personality, social interaction, and other aspects of human behavior that are perhaps not so easily discussed in terms of algorithms and other explicit models of performance.

Why do I say a lot of chess writers are more focused on psychology? A lot of them want to tell you a lot about the various habits or nervous tics of different players, for example, or encourage you to study your opponent in an almost Freudian way (Does the player fear open positions? Are they temperamentally equipped for a long positional fight?). I think a lot of these discussions are sort of seductive because they appeal to the characterization of chess as a battle of wits: Besides the objective battle happening on the board, the idea of understanding your opponent psychologically adds a less clear-cut dimension of social engineering to the game. Can you psyche your opponent out somehow? If you've researched their quirks, can you leverage that knowledge to push them into situations that you know will make them nervous and dominate them that way? I still encounter a lot of advisees in my Psychology department who love the idea of being a criminal profiler and I think some of the same things that make that career sound glamorous are at play when chess writers and readers pore over anecdotes about walking around during play vs. staying seated, or making moves with subtle gestures rather than big sweeping movements. For me, however, this isn't all that satisfying. What I'm looking for are ideas about the cognitive processes that support good chess playing - how are perception, memory, or attention trained and applied to the game? What do good players do that novices don't?

1. Why do chess players (even good ones) sometimes miss obvious replies by their opponent? - It happens all the time: You're working on your next move and you've come up with a good idea. You think about the different things your opponent might do and they all look like they lead to a good outcome for you. You wait, you think again, you nod to yourself and make the move...and your opponent promptly does something you didn't anticipate at all! Suddenly you're toast and you have no idea how you didn't see that they could just push the pawn, or recapture with the knight instead of the bishop, or threaten your queen while you attacked theirs, and on and on...

There are a lot of things to explore here I think, but the first thing it made me think of was confirmation bias, which looms large in many discussions about reasoning and decision-making in the cognitive science literature. Briefly, confirmation bias refers to the tendency to search for evidence and/or process information in a manner that supports what you believe. To put it another way, we tend to be quite bad at closely examining evidence that might prove us wrong about something. In the case of a chess game, it's obvious what the prior belief is: This move is great! Confirmation bias in this setting means that we don't go out of our way to look for responses that make the move a blunder, but instead focus on the various ways it could work out well for us.

There's a phrase I've picked up from watching IM Levy Rozman (GothamChess) that seems relevant here: "Hope Chess." This refers to playing moves and hoping that your opponent will do the thing you want rather than make the move that will leave you stuck without a path forward. Hope chess and confirmation bias are tightly coupled, and Levy's advice about how to avoid hoping in favor of playing is pretty sound - be systematic about how you consider moves and look to answer key questions like "Can they check me?" and "Can they capture something?" as specifically as possible. It would be neat to hear more about how strong players adopt strategies to mitigate confirmation bias (if they do).

2. Intuition vs. Analysis - Kotov spends a lot of time discussing how to properly calculate in chess positions, but also emphasizes that a strong player also uses intuition or judgment in many cases too. That is, rather than explicitly describing a series of moves that leads to a desired outcome, strong players often rely on pattern recognition - the ability to look at an arrangement of pieces and understand things like who is better off, which squares are strong or weak, and what kinds of things are likely to happen in the long run (e.g. "The kingside pawns will push through eventually.") I'm especially interested in this distinction between intuition and analysis because it intersects with my work in visual perception. Intuition sounds a lot like learning to recognize and label different visual stimuli, which is something I think about in my research in a bunch of different ways. A key question for me with regard to chess is what exactly is being recognized? Is it just a configuration of pieces? Is it instead the spatial transformations that a position supports?

This leads us into another idea Kotov brings up a few times, which is the difference between the static features of a position and the dynamic features. Static features are things like the material imbalances or the current positions of the pieces. Dynamic features are a little harder to define, but they refer to the ways in which pieces will be able to move either singly or in a coordinated way to transform a player's control of space, or to mount an attack on some part of the board. Kotov borrows language from physics about potential energy and kinetic energy to talk about static and dynamic features, and I'm really interested in whether and how these may be different kinds of visuospatial processing. If you watch chess streamers, a lot of them use arrows to think out loud, which suggests to me that there is perhaps an independent spatial form of processing that's maybe different than the static evaluation of what's in front of them.

I'll probably find more in his book after I read it again, but already it seems like there are a lot of neat directions for hunting through research articles and monographs. I'll be posting more soon about some of my other thoughts and explorations in this domain, so keep an eye on this space if you're interested in what chess has to tell us about cognition and vice-versa. 2351a5e196

snail bob 8 download

stm32f10x lib.h library download

british airways flight ticket download

navegador santander download

laura pausini 20 the greatest hits download free