To isolate the brain processes that underlie the selection and generation of goal directed behavior we resort to functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during delayed response tasks (see figure). These tasks allow temporally separating planning and decision processes during a "Delay" from representations of both the sensory context they depend on ("Instruction") and the motor acts they ultimately produce ("Response"). Using such task design we show at the level of human posterior parietal and premotor cortex and plans for upcoming finger reaches are encoded (Lindner et al., 2010). These plans are formed in a way that (also) considers the visual consequences of the movement (Pilacinski et al., 2018) as well as information about movement trajectories (Pilacinski & Lindner, 2019). Notably, up to a certain limit, our brain can represent multiple concurrent plans thereby warranting optimal behavioral performance (Schach et al., 2022). Given that such planning activity in posterior parietal and premotor cortex is additionally modulated by decision variables such as reward and punishment (Iyer et al., 2010), makes these areas interesting candidates for the neural monitoring of action selection (e.g. in order to inform brain machine interfaces, etc.).
A key constituent of the "Self" is "agency". Agency refers to our ability to identify whether or not our movements and their sensory consequences are caused by ourselves. Deficits of this ability do, accordingly, dissolve the border between self and non-self, as is for instance the case in certain Schizophrenia patients. A healthy subject can attribute agency to self-produced sensory information on the basis of her own motor commands: these commands allow to predict and, thus, to identify the sensory consequences of her actions. We have shown that deficits of this predictive mechanism (also termed forward model, efference copy, or corollary discharge) explain disorders of the Self in Schizophrenia (Lindner et al., 2005; Synofzik et al., 2010; Roth et al., 2023). We have further shown that sensory predictions are recalibrated by sensory feedback on a short timescale (Synofzik et al., 2006; Wilke et al., 2013). Moreover, this plasticity is mediated by the cerebellum (Lindner et al., 2006; Synofzik et al., 2008; Roth et al., 2013) and, likely, is not only optimizing perceived agency but also motor control. Using 2D virtual reality reach setups (figure; also see Methods) we currently detail the mechanisms of agency attribution, their neural underpinnings, and their disturbance in various neuropsychiatric disease (see here for preliminary results in OCD by Roth et al., 2024).
Agency attribution and its alteration in stroke patients. Julius Böhme & Karla Lauer. In collaboration with Prof. Dr. Dr. Hans-Otto Karnath, Dr. Marc Himmelbach & Dr. Matthis Synofzik, Hertie-Institute, Tübingen, Germany. Coworkers: Manuel Roth, [abstract]
The detached self: Comparing depersonalization / derealization-like experiences arising in meditation vs. other contexts. Erola Pons.
Agency, Certainty, and their Modulation through tDCS. Sarah Abdou, Nina Fischer, Eva Vennemann. In collaboration with Dr. Elisa Filewich & Dr. Simone Weller. University of Tübingen, Germany.
MEG Correlates of Agency. Remoun Georgy. In collaboration with Prof. Dr. Christoph Braun. University of Tübingen, Germany.
By recruiting “mirror” regions in the contralateral cerebral hemisphere of prefrontal cortex elderly subjects are able to master challenging cognitive tasks despite cognitive decline. Using highly demanding working memory tasks during fMRI we have shown that such bilateral prefrontal activation is not age-specific but reflects a general coping strategy that is independent of age, task content and brain region (Höller-Wallscheid et al. 2017). Preliminary evidence suggests that such cross-hemispheric recruitment is present also beyond frontal cortex and in tasks other than working memory (Schach et al. 2023). In addition we try to use the so-called “bilateral field advantage effect” as a means to externally trigger bilateral representation and to improve working memory performance despite cognitive decline.
Building on preliminary evidence (Pilacinski et al., 2020) we currently try to challenge classic conceptions of working memory: We investigate whether the way sensory information is maintained in working memory critically depends on its future use.
Improving Working Memory. Inspired by the work of Melanie Höller-Wallscheid, Teresa Kohler (& formerly also Nina Röhm) investigate by means of psychophysics and fMRI whether and how a “bilateral field advantage effect” could improve working memory performance and bilaterality. Our goal is to devise a simple behavioral strategy to support working memory in people with MCI and early dementia.
Cross-hemispheric recruitment across tasks and brain regions. Inspired by our preliminary results (Schach et al. 2023), Dominik Bauder formally tests whether cross-hemispheric recruitment is present across different cognitive tasks and across different cortical areas.
Monitoring cross-hemispheric recruitment by means of fNIRS. Annalena Mack tries to implement fNIRS for monitoring delay activity during working memory and planning, hopefully allowing us to use this method in the near future to monitor cross-hemispheric recruitment.
Decoding Working Memory. Kreshnik Bickaj. Extending the work of Pilacinski et al., 2022, we try to find evidence for verbal/spatial visual information being maintained differently in the brain, namely as dependent on whether subjects will later use the respective material for a verbal vs. a spatial finger movement response. With this we’d like to demonstrate that working memory representations consider differences in the future use of the same memory material.
The static magnetic field inside MRI-scanners induces a tonic magnetic-vestibular stimulation (MVS). MVS not only leads to a strong and persistent vestibulo-ocular reflex, which chiefly surfaces in the horizontal domain. Together with Prof. Dr. Dr. Hans-Otto Karnath and Dr. Daniel Wiesen we recently demonstrated that MVS at 3T also introduces a horizontal bias in goal directed spatial attention and behavior (Lindner et al., 2021) and that this bias sustains at least for one hour inside the scanner (Smaczny et al. 2024). Extending this collaborative research with the Karnath-lab, we currently probe whether MVS could be used to ameliorate spatial neglect after stroke (see our preprint).
Reducing spatial neglect after stroke through MVS inside a 3T MRI scanner. Prof. Dr. Dr. Hans-Otto Karnath, Hannah Rosenzopf, Britta Stammler, Stefan Smaczny, Axel Lindner. [preprint]
Magnetic Vestibular Stimulation (MVS) at 3T And Its Influence On 2D Eye Movements and Spatial Cognition. Laura Schaffeld, Carina Schaffitzel, Leonie Behle; in collaboration with Prof. Dr. Dr. Hans-Otto Karnath.
In consumer decision making there is a point at which the benefits of ever increasing choice options are outweighed by the costs of choosing from multiple alternatives, ultimately leading to choice overload. We recently demonstrated that brain activity in dorsal striatum and the anterior cingulate cortex reflect the net-benefit of increasing choice sets and speculated that this representation of choice set value motivates (or demotivates) processes of decision making (Reutskaja et al., 2018). We currently investigate whether our results generalize to other decision scenarios that may entail other types of cognitive costs and benefits.
Cognitive overload in accounting decisions. In collaboration with Prof. Dr. Burkhard Pedell, Dr. Ann Tank & Yannick Sam Schüßler, Chair of Management Accounting and Control, University of Stuttgart, Germany. [Publication of PhD Thesis] [manuscript in preparation]
Our holistic perception of the environment is a result of processes, which group the fragments of information arriving at the retina in order to form entities or objects. Studies on simultanagnosia patients, who lost the ability to perceive a visual scene holistically, emphasize the importance of parieto-occipital cortex as substrate for perceptual grouping. To study perceptual (spatial) grouping we utilize a bistable Ternus-Display that does - depending on how sensory information is grouped - lead to the perception of either element motion or group motion: Element motion (a single dot is "jumping" left and right) is commonly perceived in the upper animation of the Ternus diplay on the right while group motion (a group of three dots is "jumping" left and right) is typically perceived in the lower animation. Using this bistable stimulus in combination with fMRI, we provided preliminary evidence that parieto-occipital cortex does indeed serve as a substrate for spatial grouping (Kutscheid et al., 2014).
In a distinct line of collaborative research on perceptual decision making, we applied delayed response tasks to separate perceptual vs. motor decisions. Furthermore, our approach enabled us to trace a fine-grained functional somatosensory-to-motor cascade of cortical areas engaged in the perceptual decision making process during tactile pattern discrimination (Li Hegner et al., 2015 & 2017).
The Role of Parietal Cortex in Mediating Perceptual Grouping: Katrin Kutscheidt, Katrina R. Quinn & Sarah Trillsam. fMRI and MEG studies using a bistable Ternus display. In collaboration with Dr. Elisabeth Hein, Evolutionary Cognition Lab, Department of Psychology, Tübingen, Germany, and Prof. Dr. Cristoph Braun, MEG-Centre, Tübingen, Germany. [abstract] [manuscript in preparation]