The Universal Sense of Space, Time, and Number


John L. Waters


Use down arrow or vertical scroll bar

to view whole page!


The Universal Sense of Space, Time, and Number


John L. Waters


May 10, 2001


Copyright 2001 by John L. Waters. All Rights

Reserved

-------------------------------------------------------


Submission 3


May 10, 2001


The Universal Sense of Space, Time, and Number


The non-physicality of space and time has

made this subject very difficult to study. Take away

physical objects, and the space and time in which

physical objects are embedded is invisible. People

who have no difficulty in seeing are accustomed to

think in terms of what is visible. Moreover, those

persons who concentrate their attention on what is

visible only see physical objects at varying

distances. In compensating for a chronic handicap,

blind or partially blind persons can develop a keen

kinesthetic sense of space and time. The thesis of

this paper is that a blind or partially blind person's

perception of space and time is valid.


To go deeper into this subject, think about

the way persons move in a physical space having three

dimensions. We can move forward or backward, left or

right, and up or down. As our bodies move, we feel

the movement profoundly.


In addition, humans can look forward or

backward, left or right, and up or down. We can see

in these three different directions or dimensions.

However, some people pay a lot more attention to what

they see than what they feel in their muscles, bones,

and nerves as they travel through space.


Traveling in a car going fifty miles an

hour, we feel our bodies being moved by centrifugal

force. When the car turns to the right or to the

left, we feel the turning movement. However, many

individuals don't pay much attention to body movement.

While riding in or driving a car, normally sighted

people often concentrate on what they are looking at.


In school, young children are taught to look

at pictures, letters, and words, and to look at the

teacher. Body movement is a distraction to the other

students, so children are taught to sit still. This

training continues throughout all the twelve grades.

When bodies are not permitted to move, the sense of

body movement isn't cultivated in the children.

Eyesight is emphasized year after year after year in

school. It's important, however, to cultivate

children's body movement as well, because subtle mind

movements and subtle body movements both are subtle

intelligence in action.


Likewise, most children who see well quickly

learn to identify their peers by sight. These

children soon recognize the letters and the numerals

just by looking at them. Nearly all children develop

stereopsis, i.e., depth perception, so that they

readily judge distance by using both eyes at once.

Stereopsis makes their visual perception of distance

automatic. For example, a boy who has stereopsis

quickly learns to catch a ball by looking at the ball

with both eyes and making the appropriate body

movements. But a boy who has no depth perception

can't tell where the ball is in space as it moves

rapidly towards him. Even so, the same space exists

for the visually impaired boy as it does for all the

good ball players.


When a boy isn't blessed with stereoscopic

vision, he has to judge distance in some other way.

This requires extra effort. For example, a boy can

turn his head from side to side and observe how much

the distant object appears to move in relation to what

is behind it. The closer an object is, the more the

background seems to move behind it. A distant object

is part of the background and doesn't appear to move.

Another example judging distance without stereoptic

vision is this: the boy can remember how large a car

is, so when he sees a car that looks no larger than a

pill bug on the ground four feet below his chin, he

knows that the car is far away. When he sees a woman

that looks taller than a mature pine tree, he knows

that the woman is close by.


At every moment, vision for this boy is a

puzzle he must solve. With every scene the puzzle

changes. Therefore, a lot of the young man's brain

activity is devoted just to making sense of what he is

looking at. As long as he is awake and moving about

and interacting with other people, this puzzle-solving

activity dominates the boy's mind and body activity.

Even as a man, this person has less brain energy

available for activities which normal people enjoy and

learn to do quickly, easily, and well. The reason is

because this handicapped man's brain is having to work

so hard just so that he can avoid a serious accident.


Most objects in nature are blind. Rocks,

trees, clouds, and bodies of water have no eyes. They

can't see at all. Furthermore, the planets and the

stars are totally blind. Even so, all these objects

exist and move in space-time. Reflecting on this, we

realize that an object doesn't need eyes to exist and

move in space-time. However, normally-sighted humans

are accustomed to thinking in terms of what they can

see.


In view of the foregoing, we should realize

that there is an alternative way to think about

space-time. To experience this way, we can experiment

with ourselves. We can spend six hours wearing tight

bandages over our eyes so that we can't see at all.

We can move in spare the way a blind person does, by

reaching out with a hand or with a cane, and feeling

our way along carefully. Or we can move a long stick

back and forth in front of our feet.


It's easy to imagine a straight line

connecting your right eye to a distant flag or to a

distant star. But why is this so easy to imagine? We

observe that light travels in a straight line, and we

observe that a person can walk in a straight line

directly towards an object. We are accustomed to

judging distance by using our eyes. But a blind

person moves through space kinesthetically and is

attentive to this non-visual means of reckoning

distance.


To get from place to place, a totally blind

man rides a vehicle driven by someone else, or he

walks using a cane and perhaps a seeing eye dog. The

man can judge the distance he has walked by counting

his steps. Similarly, if we count the number of ticks

a clock makes, we can estimate the time. The walking

man and the clock are both counting numbers of units.

Both are time-keepers.


By shifting our attention to the movements

and the kinesthetic sense perceptions of a blind man,

we have arrived at the idea that counting a number of

units is the common way of perceiving and reckoning

both space and time. The idea is that number unites

both space and time.


When it comes to judging space and time, you

are an expert. You can count the number of steps you

walk from your home to the bus stop or to the market.

Furthermore, you can count the number of times the

second hand of your watch goes around the dial, and in

one minute that hand will sweep over sixty tiny marks

placed at equal intervals. You can verify this fact

again and again. Moreover, you don't need to see

stereoptically to perform this experiment.


A lot goes on inside your eyes and your

brain when you see stereoptically. Stereopsis is so

automatic that you do it without conscious effort.

However, when a person is blind in this sense, this

vision is no good for him. As far as space and time

are concerned, he is body-oriented like the Earth and

other sightless objects. And, the blind man isn't

distracted by eyesight.


Setting aside vision and depth perception by

eyesight, and considering how body movement is used to

gauge space, we begin our study of space, time, and

number. We realize that nearly everything in the

universe functions without seeing anything visually,

and so to really feel in tune with the universe, and

cultivate the sensations we feel in our whole bodies,


we stop using our eyes in the way people taught us to

use our eyes. For a time we can even put bandages

over our eyes so we don't see at all. Or we can just

learn to get around in a darkened room. This

experiment will help us become more aware of our own

body movement and experience the kinesthetic sense

more keenly.


A totally blind boy might compensate for his

sightlessness by becoming unusually sensitive in his

body. He would move his body often to keep track of

where he is in space. A boy who lacks stereoscopic

vision might compensate in the same way for the same

reason. Normally-sighted people show body movements

but people who see don't need to pay special attention

to the kinesthetic sense.


The need is to produce a model of space-time

which works for both blind objects and objects that

see. After all, binocular vision does detect distance

in physical space. Furthermore, you can see the steps

that a person takes in walking a mile. You can see

the second hand as it swings round and round the dial

of a watch or a clock. You can verify that reckoning

both space and time requires that a person count using

the natural numbers which are also called counting

numbers. Moreover, you can recognize that physical

bodies move in space-time whether they are human

bodies or not and whether they see or not. And you

can let your body move freely without preoccupied with

what is visible.


In defining space and time, our kinesthetic

sense is as valid and useful as our sense of vision.

By carefully observing the way human bodies move, and

applying the precision language which scientists use

to accurately perceive and define other phenomena, we

use both our visual sense and our kinesthetic sense to

help us define space and time in a way which is more

comprehensible and useful to people.


John L. Waters

johnlwaters@yahoo.com


The information on this page represents that of John Waters and not

necessarily that of Humboldt State University. John Waters takes full

responsibility for the information presented.


This page is maintained by: John Waters